Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The virtual reality head-mounted display Oculus Rift induces motion sickness and is sexist in its effects

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Anecdotal reports suggest that motion sickness may occur among users of contemporary, consumer-oriented head-mounted display systems and that women may be at greater risk. We evaluated the nauseogenic properties of one such system, the Oculus Rift. The head-mounted unit included motion sensors that were sensitive to users’ head movements, such that head movements could be used as control inputs to the device. In two experiments, seated participants played one of two virtual reality games for up to 15 min. In Experiment 1, 22% of participants reported motion sickness, and the difference in incidence between men and women was not significant. In Experiment 2, motion sickness was reported by 56% of participants, and incidence among women (77.78%) was significantly greater than among men (33.33%). Before participants were exposed to the head-mounted display system, we recorded their standing body sway during the performance of simple visual tasks. In both experiments, patterns of pre-exposure body sway differed between participants who (later) reported motion sickness and those who did not. In Experiment 2, sex differences in susceptibility to motion sickness were preceded by sex differences in body sway. These postural effects confirm a prediction of the postural instability theory of motion sickness. The results indicate that users of contemporary head-mounted display systems are at significant risk of motion sickness and that in relation to motion sickness these systems may be sexist in their effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen B, Hanley T, Rokers B, Green CS (2016) Visual 3D motion acuity predicts discomfort in 3D stereoscopic environments. Entertain Comput 13:1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balasubramaniam R, Riley MA, Turvey MT (2000) Specificity of postural sway to the demands of a precision task. Gait Posture 11:12–24

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnet CT, Faugloire EM, Riley MA, Bardy BG, Stoffregen TA (2006) Motion sickness preceded by unstable displacements of the center of pressure. Hum Move Sci 25:800–820

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd D (2014). Is the Oculus Rift sexist? http://qz.com/192874/is-the-oculus-rift-designed-to-be-sexist/

  • Chang C-H, Pan W-W, Tseng L-Y, Stoffregen TA (2012) Postural activity and motion sickness during video game play in children and adults. Exp Brain Res 217:299–309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen Y-C, Dong X, Chen F-C, Stoffregen TA (2012) Control of a virtual avatar influences postural activity and motion sickness. Ecol Psychol 24:279–299

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chiari L, Rocchi L, Cappello A (2002) Stabilometric parameters are affected by anthropometry and foot placement. Clin Biomech 17:666–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diels C (2014) Will autonomous vehicles make us sick? In: Sharples S, Shorrock S (eds) Contemporary ergonomics and human factors. CRC Press, Boca Raton, pp 301–307

    Google Scholar 

  • Dong X, Yoshida K, Stoffregen TA (2011) Control of a virtual vehicle influences postural activity and motion sickness. J Exp Psychol Appl 17:128–138

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Draper MH, Virre ES, Furness TA, Gawron VJ (2001) Effects of image scale and system time delay on simulator sickness within head-coupled virtual environments. Hum Fact 43:129–146

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Drummond PD (2005) Triggers of motion sickness in migraine sufferers. Headache 45:653–656

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Era P, Sainio P, Koskinen S, Haavisto P, Vaara M, Aromaa A (2006) Postural balance in a random sample of 7979 subjects aged 40 years and over. Gerontology 52:204–213

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giammarco EA, Schneider TJ, Carswell JJ, Knipe WS (2015) Video game preferences and their relation to career interests. Pers Individ Differ 73:98–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golding JF (2006) Motion sickness susceptibility. Autonom Neurosci Basic Clin 129:67–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grannell C (2013). Why iOS7 is making some users sick. http://www.theguardian.com/technology/2013/sep/27/ios-7-motion-sickness-nausea. Accessed June 23 2016

  • Ihlen EA (2012) Introduction to multifractal detrended fluctuation analysis in Matlab. Front Physiol 3:141

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ihlen EA, Vereijken B (2010) Interaction-dominant dynamics in human cognition: beyond 1/ƒ α fluctuation. J Exp Psychol Gen 139:436–463

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ihlen EA, Skjaeret N, Vereijken B (2013) The influence of center-of-mass movements on the variation in the structure of human postural sway. J Biomech 46:484–490

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelty-Stephen DG, Palatinus K, Saltzman E, Dixon JA (2013) A tutorial on multifractality, cascades, and interactivity for empirical times series in ecological science. Ecol Psychol 25:1–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy RS, Lilienthal MG (1994) Measurement and control of motion sickness after-effects from immersion in virtual reality. In: Proceedings of “Virtual Reality and Medicine: the Cutting Edge”. New York: SIG-Advanced Applications, pp 111–119

  • Kennedy RS, Dunlap WP, Fowlkes JE (1990) Prediction of motion sickness susceptibility. In: Crampton GH (ed) Motion and space sickness. CRC Press, Boca Raton FL, pp 179–215

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal MG (1993) Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int J Aviat Psychol 3:203–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy RS, Drexler J, Kennedy RC (2010) Research in visually induced motion sickness. Appl Ergonom 41:494–503

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura D (1997) Sex, sexual orientation and Sex hormones influence human cognitive functions. Biomed Rev 7:33–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klosterfalfen S, Pan F, Kellerman S, Enck P (2006) Gender and race as determinants of nausea induced by circular vection. Gend Med 3:236–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koslucher FC, Haaland E, Stoffregen TA (2014) Body load and the postural precursors of motion sickness. Gait Posture 39:606–610

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koslucher FC, Haaland E, Malsch A, Webeler J, Stoffregen TA (2015) Sex differences in the incidence of motion sickness induced by linear visual oscillation. Aviat Med Hum Perform 86:787–793

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koslucher FC, Haaland E, Stoffregen TA (2016a) Sex differences in visual performance and postural sway precede sex differences in visually induced motion sickness. Exp Brain Res 234:313–322. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4462-y

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koslucher FC, Munafo J, Stoffregen TA (2016b) Postural sway in men and women during nauseogenic motion of the illuminated environment. Exper Brain Res 234:2709–2720. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4675-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, DiZio P (2006) Space motion sickness. Exp Brain Res 175:377–399

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang DJ (2016) For virtual reality creators, motion sickness a real issue. http://phys.org/news/2016-03-virtual-reality-creators-motion-sickness.html. Accessed May 9 2016

  • Lawther A, Griffin MJ (1986) The motion of a ship at sea and the consequent motion sickness amongst passengers. Ergonomics 29:535–552

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawther A, Griffin MJ (1988) A survey of the occurrence of motion sickness amongst passengers at sea. Aviat Space Environ Med 59:399–406

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis T (2015) When will virtual-reality headsets stop making people sick? http://www.livescience.com/50129-virtual-reality-nausea-sickness.html. Accessed June 23 2016

  • Lin D, Seol H, Nussbaum MA, Madigan ML (2008) Reliability of COP-based postural sway measures and age-related differences. Gait Posture 28:337–342

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McConville KMV, Milosevic M (2014) Active video game head movement inputs. Pers Ubiq Comput 18:253–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merhi O, Faugloire E, Flanagan M, Stoffregen TA (2007) Motion sickness, console video games, and head mounted displays. Hum Fact 49:920–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munafo J, Curry C, Wade MG, Stoffregen TA (2016) The distance of visual targets affects the spatial magnitude and multifractal scaling of standing body sway in younger and older adults. Exp Brain Res 234:2721–2730

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Read JCA, Bohr I (2014) User experience while viewing stereoscopic 3D television. Ergonomics 57:1140–1153

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Regan EC, Price KR (1994) The frequency of occurrence and severity of side-effects of immersion virtual reality. Aviat Space Environ Med 65:527–530

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riccio GE, Stoffregen TA (1991) An ecological theory of motion sickness and postural instability. Ecol Psychol 3:195–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolnick A, Lubow RE (1991) Why is the driver rarely motion sick? The role of controllability in motion sickness. Ergonomics 34:867–879

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanney K, Salvendy G, Deisinger J, DiZio P, Ellis S, Ellision J, Fogleman G, Gallimore J, Hettinger L, Kennedy R, Lackner J, Lawson B, Maida J, Mead A, Mon-Williams M, Newman D, Piantanida T, Reeves L, Riedel O, Singer M, Stoffregen T, Wann J, Welch R, Wilson J, Witmer R (1998) Aftereffects and sense of presence in virtual environments: formulation of a research and development agenda. Report sponsored by the Life Sciences Division at NASA headquarters. Int J Hum Comput Interact 10:135–187

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SC, Parsons MG (2002) Effects of motion at sea on crew performance: a survey. Mar Technol 39:29–47

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA (1985) Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. J Exper Psychol: Human Percept Perf 11:554–565

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ (1998) Postural instability precedes motion sickness. Brain Res Bull 47:437–448

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Hettinger LJ, Haas MW, Roe M, Smart LJ (2000a) Postural instability and motion sickness in a fixed-base flight simulator. Hum Fact 42:458–469

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Pagulayan RJ, Bardy BG, Hettinger LJ (2000b) Modulating postural control to facilitate visual performance. Hum Move Sci 19:203–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Faugloire E, Yoshida K, Flanagan M, Merhi O (2008) Motion sickness and postural sway in console video games. Hum Fact 50:322–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Yoshida K, Villard S, Scibora L, Bardy BG (2010) Stance width influences postural stability and motion sickness. Ecol Psychol 22:169–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Chen F-C, Varlet M, Alcantara C, Bardy BG (2013) Getting your sea legs. PLOS ONE 8(6):e66949. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066949

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Chen Y-C, Koslucher FC (2014) Motion control, motion sickness, and the postural dynamics of mobile devices. Exp Brain Res 232:1389–1397

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner M (1999) Motion sickness in public road transport: passenger behaviour and susceptibility. Ergonomics 42:444–461

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner M, Griffin MJ (1999) Motion sickness in public road transport: the relative importance of motion, vision, and individual differences. Br J Psychol 90:519–530

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol Bull 117:250–270

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We offer our grateful thanks to Bernd Kreimeier, for his support and for the loan of the DK-2, to Zachary Pohlkamp, for assistance with data collection, and to Shotah D. Stoffregen, for assistance in evaluating and selecting experimental stimuli. Meg Diedrick’s participation was supported by an award from the Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program. Justin Munafo’s participation was supported by a fellowship from the Diversity of Views and Experiences (DOVE) Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas A. Stoffregen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Munafo, J., Diedrick, M. & Stoffregen, T.A. The virtual reality head-mounted display Oculus Rift induces motion sickness and is sexist in its effects. Exp Brain Res 235, 889–901 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4846-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4846-7

Keywords

Navigation