Skip to main content

The Consequences of Deafness for Spoken Language Development

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Deafness

Part of the book series: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research ((SHAR,volume 47))

  • 1946 Accesses

Abstract

Untreated prelinguistic deafness has a major effect on the development of spoken language, slowing development to less than half the normal rate on average, and often resulting in permanent spoken language deficits. When treated with cochlear implants at a reasonably early age, profoundly deaf children can learn spoken language, following patterns and stages of development that are similar to the patterns and stages for children with normal hearing and children with moderate and severe hearing loss who wear hearing aids. For example, receptive language and expressive language progress at similar rates, and vocabulary expands in step with other aspects of language such as phonology, morphology, syntax, and pragmatics. There appears to be a critical level of pure tone average hearing loss at about 90 dB HL that separates groups of children who are qualitatively different in the way they use their hearing and language skills. Cochlear implants are capable of moving children from the “deaf” to the “hard-of-hearing” group. Early intervention with cochlear implants or hearing aids and structured language learning has a very positive effect on the early rate of spoken language development, but there is a very wide range of spoken language outcomes in children when they reach the age of nine or ten years and it is not clearly established whether the outcomes are primarily related to age at intervention, environmental factors, cognitive abilities of the child, or the time devoted to language learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allen, S.E.M. & Crago, M.B. (1996). Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language, 23(1), 129–155.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American National Standards Institute (ANSI). (1996). Specifications for audiometers (ANSI S3.6-1996). New York: ANSI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archbold, S. M., Nikolopoulos, T. P., Tait, M., O’Donoghue, G. M., Lutman, M. E., & Gregory, S. (2000). Approach to communication, speech perception and intelligibility after paediatric cochlear implantation. British Journal of Audiology, 34(4), 257–264.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bamford, J., & Saunders, E. (1991). Hearing impairment, auditory perception, and language disability. London: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-Chava, Y., Martin, D., & Kosciw, J. G. (2005). Longitudinal improvements in communication and socialization of deaf children with cochlear implants and hearing aids: Evidence from parental reports. The Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(12), 1287–1296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. S. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 313–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, P. J. (1991). Comparison of mothers’ and fathers’ perceptions of the effect of young children with and without disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95(5), 585–595.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. B. (1986). Mothers’ internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 247–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bench, R. J., & Bamford, J. (1979). Speech-hearing tests and the spoken language of hearing-impaired children. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettes, B. A. (1988). Maternal depression and motherese: Temporal and intonational features. Child Development, 59, 1089–1096.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., & Sarant, J. (2002). Speech perception and language criteria for pediatric cochlear implant candidature. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 7, 114–121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., & Sarant, J. Z. (2011). Development of spoken language by deaf children. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education, 2nd ed. (pp. 241–257). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., Barry, J. G., & Jacq, P. (2001a). Phonetic inventory development in young cochlear implant users 6 years postoperation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44(1), 73–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., Sarant, J. Z., Paatsch, L. E., Barry, J. G., Bow, C. P., Wales, R. J., Wright, M., Psarros, C., Rattigan, K., & Tooher, R. (2001b). Relationships among speech perception, production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with impaired hearing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(2), 264–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., Barry, J., Bow, C., Sarant, J., Paatsch, L., & Wales, R. (2001c). The development of speech production following cochlear implantation. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 15(5), 363–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blamey, P. J., Paatsch, L. E., Bow, C. P., Sarant, J. Z., & Wales, R. J. (2002). A critical level of hearing for speech perception in children. Acoustics Research Letters Online, 3(1), 18–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, A. (1997). Auditory capacity of hearing-impaired children using hearing aids and cochlear implants: Issues of efficacy and assessment. Scandanavian Audiology Supplementum, 46, 17–25.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, A., & Eran, O. (1994). Auditory speech perception capacity of child implant users expressed as equivalent hearing loss. Volta Review, 96(5), 151–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, A., Geers, A. E., & Moog, J. S. (1991). Practical implications of cochlear implants in children. Ear and Hearing, 12(4 Supplement), 81S–89S.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1974). Discussion summary—Development of concepts underlying language. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives—acquisition, retardation, and intervention. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brackett, D. (1983). Group communication strategies for the hearing impaired. In R. R. Truax & J. J. Shultz (Eds.), Learning to communicate: Implications for the hearing impaired. Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Sherman, T. (1987). Speech patterning of natural discourse of well and depressed mothers and their young children. Child Development, 58(2), 395–400.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, P. L., Frost, B. J., Mason, J. L., & Chung, K. (1985). Acquisition of a 250-word vocabulary through a tactile vocoder. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77(4), 1576–1579.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). Development of the first language in the human species. American Psychologist, 28(2), 97–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Nabors, L. A., & Bryant, D. M. (1996). Quality of center child care and infant cognitive and language development. Child Development, 67(2), 606–620.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Riggin, R., Jr., Zeisel, S. A., Neebe, E., & Bryant, D. (2000). Relating quality of center-based child care to early cognitive and language development longitudinally. Child Development, 71(2), 339–357.Clark, E. (1991). Acquisitional principles in lexical development. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 31–71). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, G. M., Blamey, P. J., Busby, P. A., Dowell, R. C., Franz, B. K., Musgrave, G. N., Nienhuys, T. G., Pyman, B. C., Roberts, S. A., & Tong, Y. C. (1987). A multiple-electrode intracochlear implant for children. Archives of Otolaryngology—Head & Neck Surgery, 113(8), 825–828.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S. E., & Beckwith, L. (1976). Maternal language in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 12(4), 371–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, L. (1986). Oralism in perspective. In D. Luterman (Ed.), Deafness in perspective. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, C. M., & Zwolan, T. A. (2004). Examining multiple sources of influence on the reading comprehension skills of children who use cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47(3), 509–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, C. M., Hieber, S., Arts, H. A., & Zwolan, T. A. (2000). Speech, vocabulary, and the education of children using cochlear implants: Oral or total communication? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43(5), 1185–1204.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, C. M., Craig, H. K., Raudenbush, S. W., Heavner, K., & Zwolan, T. A. (2006). The age at which young deaf children receive cochlear implants and their vocabulary and speech-production growth: Is there an added value for early implantation? Ear & Hearing, (27), 628–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. A., & Flowers, C. R. (1987). Children with a history of acquired aphasia: Residual language and academic impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(3), 251–262.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coppieters, R. (1987). Competence differences between native and near-native speakers. Language, 63(3), 544–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornett, R. O. (1985). Diagnostic factors bearing on the use of cued speech with hearing-impaired children. Ear and Hearing, 6(1), 33–35.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crago, M. B., & Annahatak, B. (1993). Changing patterns of language socialization in Inuit homes. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 24(3), 205–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosson, J., & Geers, A. (2001). Analysis of narrative ability in children with cochlear implants. Ear & Hearing, 22(5), 381–394.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (1994). Maternal depression and child development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(1), 73–112.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: a psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild child.” New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. S., Geers, A. E., Blamey, P. J., Tobey, E. A., & Brenner, C. A. (2011). Factors contributing to speech perception scores in long-term pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear & Hearing, 32(1 Supplement), 19S–26S.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, K. (1947). Final note on a case of extreme isolation. American Journal of Sociology, 52(5), 432–437.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H., & Silverman, S. R. (1978). Hearing and deafness, 4th ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, P. W., Blamey, P. J., Dettman, S. J., Barker, E. J., & Clark, G. M. (1995). A clinical report on receptive vocabulary skills in cochlear implant users. Ear and Hearing, 16(3), 287–294.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208(4448), 1174–1176.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, H. (2001). Hearing aids. Sydney: Boomerang Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodd, B., McIntosh, B., & Woodhouse, L. (1998). Early lipreading ability and speech and language development of hearing-impaired pre-schoolers. In R. Campbell, B. Dodd, & D. K. Burnham (Eds.), Hearing by eye II: Advances in the psychology of speechreading and auditory-visual speech. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doupe, A. J., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. The Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567–631.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Duchesne, L., Sutton, A., & Bergeron, F. (2009). Language achievement in children who received cochlear implants between 1 and 2 years of age: Group trends and individual patterns. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(4), 465–485.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. T. (1997). PPVT-III: Peabody picture vocabulary test, 3rd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupuy, H. J. (1974). The rationale, development, and standardization of a basic word vocabulary test: A methodological report on the conceptual representation and measurement of American-English basic word vocabulary acquisition. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Health Resources Administration, National Center for Health Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. C. (2007). Children with cochlear implants and complex needs: A review of outcome research and psychological practice. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(3), 258–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, L. S., Berliner, K. I., Thielemeir, M. A., Kirk, K. I., & Tiber, N. (1983). Cochlear implants in children. Ear and Hearing, 4(1), 41–50.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El-Hakim, H., Levasseur, J., Papsin, B. C., Panesar, J., Mount, R. J., Stevens, D., & Harrison, R. V. (2001). Assessment of vocabulary development in children after cochlear implantation. Archives of Otolaryngology—Head & Neck Surgery, 127(9), 1053–1059.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, N. (1975). Auditory-visual perception of speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 40, 481–492.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischel, J. E., Whitehurst, G. J., Caulfield, M. B., & DeBaryshe, B. (1989). Language growth in children with expressive language delay. Pediatrics, 82(2), 218–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, W. (1995). Language interaction techniques for stimulating the development of at risk children in infant and preschool day care. Early Child Development and Care, 111(1), 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., & Wessels, J. M. (1993). Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(3), 287–295.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. E. (2002). Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33(3), 172–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A., & Moog, J. (1989). Factors predictive of the development of literacy in profoundly hearing-impaired adolescents. Volta Review, 91(2), 69–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A., & Moog, J. (1994). Spoken language results: Vocabulary, syntax, and communication. Volta Review Monograph, 96(5), 131–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A., Tobey, E., Moog, J., & Brenner, C. (2008). Long-term outcomes of cochlear implantation in the preschool years: From elementary grades to high school. International Journal of Audiology, 47(Supplement 2), S21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geffner, D. (1987). The development of language in young hearing-impaired children. ASHA Monographs, 26, 25–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1985). Development of print awareness in language-disordered preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 521–526.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17(1), 171–183.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goppold, L. (1988). Early intervention for preschool deaf children: The longitudinal academic effects relative to program methodology. American Annals of the Deaf, 133(4), 285–288.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goto, H. (1971). Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of the sounds “l” and “r,” Neuropsychologia, 9(3), 317–323.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grace, S. L., Evindar, A., & Stewart, D. E. (2003). The effect of postpartum depression on child cognitive development and behavior: A review and critical analysis of the literature. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 6(4), 263–274.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grimwood, K., Anderson, P., Anderson, V., Tan, L., & Nolan, T. (2000). Twelve year outcomes following bacterial meningitis: Further evidence for persisting effects. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 83(2), 111–116.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallé, P. A., & de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1994). Emergence of an early receptive lexicon: Infants’ recognition of words. Infant Behavior and Development, 17(2), 119–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1979). One child’s protolanguage. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of interpersonal communication (pp. 171–190). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., Jones, D., Brookes, S., & Grant, J. (1986). Relations between the non-verbal context of maternal speech and rate of language development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 261–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay-McCutcheon, M. J., Kirk, K. I., Henning, S. C., Gao, S., & Qi, R. (2008). Using early language outcomes to predict later language ability in children with cochlear implants. Audiology & Neurotology, 13(6), 370–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(4), 271–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(4), 603–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M. E. (1983). Verbal interaction between mothers and their young hearing-impaired children. Journal of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf, 7, 18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. (1993). How word recognition may evolve from infant speech perception capacities. In G. T. M. Altmann & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Cognitive models of speech processing: The Second Sperlonga Meeting (pp. 22–55). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koluchova, J. (1972). Severe deprivation in twins: A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13(2), 107–114.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., Heid, S., & Klinke, R. (2001). Delayed maturation and sensitive periods in the auditory cortex. Audiology & Neurotology, 6(6), 346–362.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kretschmer, R. R., & Kretschmer, L. W. (1986). Language in perspective. In D. M. Luterman (Ed.), Deafness in perspective (pp. 131–166). San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5, 831–843.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255(5044), 606–608.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E. (1997). Nonparental child care: Context, quality, correlates, and consequences. In I. E. Sigel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 4: Child psychology in practice (pp. 73–174). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamoré, P. J., Verweij, C., & Brocaar, M. P. (1990). Residual hearing capacity of severely hearing-impaired subjects. Acta Oto-laryngologica Supplementum, 469, 7–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, H. (1995). The dispute concerning the benefits to be expected from cochlear implantation of young deaf children. American Journal of Otology, 16(3), 393–399.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, H., McGarr, N., & Geffner, D. (1987). Concluding commentary. ASHA Monographs, 26, 140–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, E. H. (1998). The relationships between reading processes and English skills of deaf college students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3(2), 80–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Limbrick, E. A., McNaughton, S., & Clay, M. M. (1992). Time engaged in reading: A critical factor in reading achievement. American Annals of the Deaf, 137(4), 309–314.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ling, D. (1976). Speech and the hearing-impaired child: Theory and practice. Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ling, D. (1989). Foundations of spoken language for hearing-impaired children. Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacTurk, R. H., Meadow-Orlans, K. P., Koester, L. S., & Spencer, P. E. (1993). Social support, motivation, language, and interaction—a longitudinal-study of mothers and deaf infants. American Annals of the Deaf, 138(1), 19–25.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahshie, S. N. (1995). Educating deaf children bilingually : With insights and applications from Sweden and Denmark. Washington, DC: Pre-College Programs, Gallaudet University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marschark, M., Lang, H. G., & Albertini, J. A. (2002). Educating deaf students: From research to practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R. I. (1994). The importance of childhood to language acquisition: Evidence from American Sign Language. In J. C. Goodman & H. C. Nusbaum (Eds.), The development of speech perception: The transition from speech sounds to spoken words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of language: The study of developmental psycholinguistics. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amieltison, C. (1988). A precursor of language-acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29(2), 143–178.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1970). Measures of vocabulary and grammatical skills for children up to age six. Developmental Psychology, 2(3), 439–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, M. P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, 106(3), e43.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, M. P., Tomblin, J. B., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Connor, C. M., & Jerger, S. (2007). Current state of knowledge: Language and literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear and Hearing, 28(6), 740–753.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moog, J. S., & Geers, A. E. (2003). Epilogue: Major findings, conclusions and implications for deaf education. Ear and Hearing, 24(1 Supplement), 121S–121S.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 495–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moores, D. F. (1987). Educating the deaf: Psychology, principles, and practices. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moores, D. F., & Sweet, C. (1990). Relationships of English grammar and communicative fluency to reading in deaf adolescents. Exceptionality, 1(2), 97–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moores, D. F., Weiss, K. L., & Goodwin, M. W. (1978). Early education-programs for hearing-impaired children—major findings. American Annals of the Deaf, 123(8), 925–936.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E., Greenberg, M. T., Booth, C. L., & Spieker, S. J. (1990). Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 2(02), 127–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myklebust, H. R. (1964). The psychology of deafness: Sensory deprivation, learning, and adjustment. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholas, J. G., & Geers, A. E. (2007). Will they catch up? The role of age at cochlear implantation in the spoken language development of children with severe to profound hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1048–1062.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niparko, J. K., Tobey, E. A., Thal, D. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Wang, N-Y, Quittner, A. L., & Fink, N. E. (2010). Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA, 303(15), 1498–1506.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, S. J., Gorga, M. P., Widen, J. E., Folsom, R. C., Sininger, Y., Cone-Wesson, B., Vohr, B. R., & Fletcher, K. A. (2000). Identification of neonatal hearing impairment: Summary and recommendations. Ear and Hearing, 21(5), 529–535.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. C., & Farran, D. C. (1982). Effects of day-care experience on the use of intentional communicative behaviors in a sample of socio-economically depressed infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(1), 22–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osberger, M. J., Moeller, M. P., Eccarius, M., Robbins, A. M., Johnson, D., & Association, A. S.-L.-H. (1986). Expressive language skills. In M. J. Osberger (Ed.), Language and learning skills of hearing-impaired students (pp. 54–65). Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oshima-Takane, Y. & Robbins, M. (2003). Linguistic environment of secondborn children. First Language, 23(1), 21–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owens, R. E. (1992). Language development: An introduction, 3rd ed. New York: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paatsch, L. E., Blamey, P. J., Sarant, J. Z., Martin, L. F. A., & Bow, C. P. (2004). Separating contributions of hearing, lexical knowledge, and speech production, to speech perception scores in children with hearing impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 738–750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palermo, D. S., & Molfese, D. L. (1972). Language acquisition from age five onward. Psychological Bulletin, 78(6), 409–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, P. V., & Quigley, S.P. (1994). Language and deafness. San Diego: Singular.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peloquin, L. J., & Davidson, P. W. (1988). Psychological sequelae of pediatric infectious diseases. In D. K. Routh (Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plant, G. (1998). Training in the use of a tactile supplement to lipreading: A long-term case study. Ear and Hearing, 19(5), 394–406.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plomp, R. (1978). Auditory handicap of hearing impairment and the limited benefit of hearing aids. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63(2), 533–549.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Power, D. J., Wood, D. J., Wood, H. A., & MacDougall, J. (1990). Maternal control over conversations with hearing and deaf infants and young children. First Language, 10(28), 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prater, M. A. (1992). Increasing time-on-task in the classroom: Suggestions for improving the amount of time learners spend in on-task behaviors. Intervention in School and Clinic, 28(1), 22–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 544–557.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pye, C. (1986). Quiche Mayan speech to children. Journal of Child Language, 13(1), 85–100.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pyman, B., Blamey, P., Lacy, P., Clark, G., & Dowell, R. (2000). The development of speech perception in children using cochlear implants: Effects of etiologic factors and delayed milestones. American Journal of Otology, 21(1), 57–61.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raffin, M. J. M., Davis, J. M., & Gilman, L. A. (1978). Comprehension of inflectional morphemes by deaf-children exposed to a visual English sign system. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21(2), 387–400.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rance, G., Barker, E. J., Sarant, J. Z., & Ching, T. Y. C. (2007). Receptive language and speech production in children with auditory neuropathy/dyssynchrony type hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 28(5), 694–702.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1948). The comparative assessment of intellectual ability. British Journal of Psychology, 39, 12–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhea, P., & Elwood, T.J. (1991). Maternal linguistic input to toddlers and slow expressive language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 982–988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rickards, F. W., Tan, L. E., Cohen, L. T., Wilson, O. J., Drew, J. H., & Clark, G. M. (1994). Auditory steady-state evoked-potential in newborns. British Journal of Audiology, 28(6), 327–337.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinshaw, H. M. (1994). Deaf infants, early intervention and language acquisition. Early Child Development and Care, 99(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinshaw, H. M., & Evans, R. (1996). Assessing the acquisition of the auditory, communicative and linguistic skills of a congenitally deaf infant pre- and post-cochlear implications. Journal of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf, 20, 8–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruben, R. J. (1992). The ontogeny of human hearing. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 112(2), 192–196.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santarcangelo, S., & Dyer, K. (1988). Prosodic aspects of motherese: Effects on gaze and responsiveness in developmentally disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46(3), 406–418.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarant J. Z. (2012). Cochlear implants in children: A review. In S. Naz (Ed.), Hearing loss, US: InTech. Available at:http://www.intechopen.com/books/hearing-loss/cochlear-implants-in-children-a-review

  • Sarant, J. Z., Blamey, P. J., Dowell, R. C., Clark, G. M., & Gibson, W. P. R. (2001). Variation in speech perception scores among children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 22(1), 18–28.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarant, J. Z., Holt, C. M., Dowell, R. C., Rickards, F. W., & Blamey, P. J. (2009). Spoken language development in oral preschool children with permanent childhood deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(2), 205–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarant, J. Z., Hughes, K., & Blamey, P. J. (2010). The effect of IQ on spoken language and speech perception development in children with impaired hearing. Cochlear Implants International, 11(Supplement 1), 370–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., &Weinberg, R. A. (1978). The influence of “family background” on intellectual attainment. American Sociological Review, 43(5), 674–692.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schafer, G., & Plunkett, K. (1998). Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. Child Development, 69(2), 309–320.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schildroth, A. N. (1994). Congenital cytomegalovirus and deafness. American Journal of Audiology, 3(2), 27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schorr, E. A., Roth, F. P., & Fox, N. A. (2008). A comparison of the speech and language skills of children with cochlear implants and children with normal hearing. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 29(4), 195–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1995). CELF 3: Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp., Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., & Spahr, A. J. (2002). A sensitive period for the development of the central auditory system in children with cochlear implants: Implications for age of implantation. Ear and Hearing, 23(6), 532–539.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., & Kral, A. (2005). The influence of a sensitive period on central auditory development in children with unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants. Hearing Research, 203(1–2), 134–143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, R. K., & Hardie, N. A. (2001). Deafness-induced changes in the auditory pathway: Implications for cochlear implants. Audiology & Neuro-Otology, 6(6), 305–318.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, M. D. (1986). From birth to five years: Children’s developmental progress. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, M. W. (1988). Hearing aid evaluation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, L., Springer, S., & Vachon, M. L. S. (1993). Disordered communication and grieving in deaf member families. Family Process, 32(2), 171–183.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Scaramella, L. V. (2006). Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9(1), 65–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, P. E., Marshark, M., & Spencer, L. J. (2011). Cochlear implants: Advances, issues, and implications. In M. Marshark & P. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoel-Gammon, C., & Otomo, K. (1986). Babbling development of hearing-impaired and normally hearing subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51(1), 33–41.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, P. (1988). Blueprint for developing conversational competence: A planning/instruction model with detailed scenarios. Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svirsky, M. A., Teoh, S. W., & Neuburger, H. (2004). Development of language and speech perception in congenitally, profoundly deaf children as a function of age at cochlear implantation. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 9(4), 224–233.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanksley, C. K. (1993). Interactions between mothers and normal-hearing or hearing-impaired children. The Volta Review, 95(1), 33–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Termine, N. T., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Infants’ responses to their mothers’ expressions of joy and sadness. Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 223–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoutenhoofd, E. (2006). Cochlear implanted pupils in Scottish schools: 4-year school attainment data (2000–2004). Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11(2), 171–188.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tobey, E. A., Rekart, D., Buckley, K., & Geers, A. E. (2004). Mode of communication and classroom placement impact on speech intelligibility. Archives of Otolaryngology—Head & Neck Surgery, 130(5), 639–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Barker, B. A., Spencer, L. J., Zhang, X. Y., & Gantz, B. J. (2005). The effect of age at cochlear implant initial stimulation on expressive language growth in infants and toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48(4), 853–867.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uziel, A. S., Sillon, M., Vieu, A., Artieres, F., Piron, J. P., Daures, J. P., & Mondain, M. (2007). Ten-year follow-up of a consecutive series of children with multichannel cochlear implants. Otology & Neurotology, 28(5), 615–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Lely, H. K. J. (1993). Specific language impairment in children: Research findings and their therapeutic implications. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 28(3), 247–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wada, J. A., & Davis, A. E. (1977). Fundamental nature of human infants brain asymmetry. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 4(3), 203–207.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F. (1994). Cross-language speech perception: Development change does not involve loss. In J. Goodman & H. C. Nusbaum (Eds.), The development of speech perception: The transition from speech sounds to spoken words. London: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 7(1), 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. J., & White, R. E. (1987). The effects of hearing status of the family and age of intervention on receptive and expressive oral language skills in hearing-impaired infants. ASHA Monographs, 26, 9–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (1992). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals–preschool (CELF-P). San Antonio, TX The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13-month-olds and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 553–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1999). Benefits of early intervention for children with hearing loss. Otolaryngologic Clinics of North America, 32(6), 1089–1102.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2003). Early intervention after universal neonatal hearing screening: Impact on outcomes. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9(4), 252–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., Coulter, D. K., & Mehl, A. L. (1999). Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics, 102(5), 1161–1171.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Although the authors of this chapter take full responsibility, they wish to acknowledge that the contents of this chapter have been developed over 32 years of research collaboration with colleagues at the University of Melbourne, the Bionics Institute (formerly the Bionic Ear Institute), and elsewhere nationally and internationally. In particular the authors thank the following people for their insights and friendship over the years: Emily Tobey, Ann Geers, Jean Moog, Lisa Davidson, Karyn Galvin, Peter Busby, Roger Wales, Johanna Barry, Dimity Dornan, Graeme Clark, Richard Dowell, Louise Paatsch, Janet Fletcher, Daniel Ling, Elaine Saunders, Shani Dettman, Margo Skinner, and Field Rickards. The first author (Peter Blamey) was trained as a nuclear physicist and actuary before 1979, and is consequently unlikely to have known anything at all about language development in children with impaired hearing if it were not for the patient assistance of the aforementioned colleagues. The authors’ research has been funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council, the Australian Research Council, the Lions International Deafness Research Fellowship, and other philanthropic sources. The Bionics Institute acknowledges the support it receives from the Victorian Government through its Operational Infrastructure Support Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter J. Blamey .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Blamey, P.J., Sarant, J.Z. (2013). The Consequences of Deafness for Spoken Language Development. In: Kral, A., Popper, A., Fay, R. (eds) Deafness. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, vol 47. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/2506_2013_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics