Skip to main content

Behavioral Aspects of Predation

  • Chapter
Perspectives in Ethology

Abstract

In this paper, I discuss four hypotheses concerning mechanisms which might be of general importance in the behavior of predators: hunting by searching image, hunting by expectation, area-restricted search, and “niche” hunting. In addition, I review briefly two approaches which have been taken in studying predator behavior at a more general level: experimental component analysis and optimal foraging theory. I make no attempt to review comprehensively the literature on behavior of predators.

The concept of hunting by searching image has been used in a great variety of senses, and to be of value it should be used in a more restricted manner. Searching-image formation in the strict sense of “learning to see” has been demonstrated in several laboratory experiments and perhaps in some seminatural field experiments with birds. The real problem is that it is difficult to distinguish in operational terms between “learning to see” and some other phenomena which result in the predator’s preferring one prey type over another. This is especially true in the field; thus the value of the concept of “learning to see” in field studies must be questioned. Gibb’s hypothesis of hunting by expectation still needs to be tested in a critical way. Although the hypothesis was put forward in the context of birds hunting for insect larvae in pine cones, essentially the same problem confronts any predator hunting for clumped prey, namely, when to leave one clump and go on to the next one. Some alternative hypotheses are discussed. Area-restricted search after capturing a prey item seems to be a fairly widespread phenomenon. At least some predators can learn to alter their searching path to improve their exploitation of particular dispersions of prey. The hypothesis of “niche” hunting is supported by some laboratory studies and indirectly by several pieces of field evidence. Experimental component analysis is an attempt to investigate and synthesize all the behavioral factors underlying the response of a predator to changes in prey density. This approach has been more concerned with determining the existence and effect of particular behavioral processes than with elucidating the mechanisms underlying each process. So far it has only been used in studying the behavior of predators in simple laboratory situations. Optimal foraging models have provided useful hypotheses for looking at particular mechanisms. More general models seem to make either very “weak” predictions about behavioral mechanisms (ones which could be produced by many other hypotheses) or ones which are not easily testable.

Financial support was provided by grant N.R.C. 67–6295.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allen, J. A. (1972). Evidence for stabilizing and apostatic selection by wild blackbirds. Nature (Lond.) 237: 348–349.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, J. A., and Clarke, B. (1968). Evidence for apostatic selection by wild passerines. Nature (Land.) 220: 501–502.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, R. R. (1970). Bird predation as a selective pressure on the immature stages of the cabbage butterflies Pieris rapae and P. brasscae. J. Zool. Land. 162: 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, C. J. (1957). The behaviour of individual coccinellid larvae on plants. Brit. J. Aninz. Behay. 5: 12–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bansch, R. (1966). On prey-seeking behaviour of aphidophagous insects. In Hodek, I. (ed.), Ecology of Aphidophagous Insects, Academia, Prague.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beukema, J. J. (1968). Predation by the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.): The influence of hunger and experience. Behaviour 31: 1–126.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bitterman, M. E. (1965). Phyletic difference in learning. Am. Psychologist 20: 396–410.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bitterman, M. E., Tyler, D. W., and Elam, C. B. (1958). Some comparative psychology. Am. J. Psychol. 71: 94–110.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1965). Information processing in the nervous system. Science 150: 457–462.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brower, J. V. Z. (1963). Experimental studies and new evidence on the evolution of mimicry. Proc. 16th Internat. Zool. Congr. 3–4: 156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. G. B. (1969). Seed selection by pigeons. Behaviour 34: 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, J. E. (1971). Prey specialization by individual trout living in a stream and pond: Some effects of feeding history and parental stock on food choice. Ph. D. thesis, University of British Columbia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, T. (1953). Effects of temperature and parasite density on the rate of increase of an insect parasite. Ecology 34: 322–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, T. (1958). Effect of host distribution on the reproduction of Encarsia formosa Gahan (Hymenoptera: Chalcidoidae). Can. Entomol. 90: 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capretta, P. J. (1969). The establishment of food preferences in chicks Gallus gallus. Anim. Behay. 17: 229–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, A. E. F. (1969). Locomotory behaviour of first instar larvae of aphidophagous Syrphidae (Diptera) after contact with aphids. Anim. Behay. 17: 673–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. L. (1973). Optimal foraging theory: some theoretical explorations. Ph. D. thesis, University of Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, R. B. (1962). Balanced polymorphism and the diversity of species. In Nichols, D. (ed.),,Taxonomy and Geography, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, R. B. (1969). The evidence for apostatic selection. Heredity 24: 347–352.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cody, M. L. (1971). Finch flocks in the Mohave Desert. Theoret. Popul. Biol. 2: 142–148.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R. F. (1969). The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behavior with reference to the evolution of warning colouration in butterflies. Behaviour 35: 45–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R. P. (1970). The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behaviour with reference to the evolution of warning coloration in butterflies. II. Reactions of naive birds to novel insects. Am. Naturalist 104: 323–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croze, H. J. (1970). Searching images in carrion crows. Z. Tierpsychol. Beiheft. 5: 85 pp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curio, E. (1966). Die Schutzanpassungen dreier Raupen eines Schwärmers (Sphingidae) auf Galapogos. Zool. Jahrb. Syst. 91: 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. (1970). The mechanism of hunting by “searching image” in birds. D. Phil. thesis, Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. (1971a). Perceptual changes in chicks: Another look at the “search image” concept. Anim. Behay. 19: 566–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. (1971b). Shifts of “attention” in chicks during feeding. Anim. Behay. 19: 575–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Den Boer, M. H. (1971). A colour polymorphism in caterpillars of Bupalus piniarius (L.) (Lepidoptera: Geometridae). Netherlands J. Zool. 21: 61–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Ruiter, L. (1952). Some experiments on the camouflage of stick caterpillars. Behaviour 4: 222–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Ruiter, L. (1955). Countershading in caterpillars. Arch. Néerl. Zool. 11: 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Ruiter, L. (1967). The feeding behaviour of vertebrates in their natural environment. In Handbook of Physiology, Sect. 6: Alimentary Canal, Vol. I: Food and Water Intake, American Physiological Society, Washington, D. C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dill, L. M. (1972). Prey avoidance learning and the functional response of predators. Ph. D. thesis, University of British Columbia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, A. F. G. (1959). An experimental study of the searching behaviour of the predatory coccinellid beetle Adalia decempunctata (L). J. Anim. Ecol. 28: 259–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, J. M. (1966). The role of time and energy in food preference. Am. Naturalist 100: 611–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, J. M. (1968). Optimal choice in animals. Am. Naturalist 102: 385–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, J. M. (1973). Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, Addison Wesley, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, F. L. (1971). Visual conspicuity, directed attention and retinal locus. Vision Res. 11: 563–576.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, E. B. (1964). Ecological Genetics, Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibb, J. A. (1958). Predation by tits and squirrels on the eucosmid Ernarmonia conicolana (Heyl.). J. Anim. Ecol. 27: 375–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibb, J. A. (1962a). L. Tinbergen’s hypothesis of the role of specific search images. Ibis 104: 106–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibb, J. A. (1962b). Tits and their food supply in English pine woods: A problem in applied ornithology. Festschr. Vogelschutzwarte, Hessen, Rheinland-Pfalz and Saarland, pp. 58–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibb, J. A. (1966). Tit predation and the abundance of Ernarmonia conicolana (Heyl.) on Weeting Heath, Norfolk, 1962–63. J. Anim. Ecol. 35: 43–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goss-Custard, J. D. (1970a). The responses of redshank (Tringa totanus (L)) to spatial variations in their prey density. J. Anim. Ecol. 39: 91–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goss-Custard, J. D. (1970b). Factors affecting the diet and feeding rates of the redshank (Tringa totanus) Symp. Brit. Ecol. Soc. 10: 101–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, K. S., and Holling, C. S. (1969). A competition submodel for parasites and predators. Can. Entomol. 101: 785–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassell, M. P. (1971a). Mutual interference between searching insect parasites. J. Anim. Ecol. 40: 473–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassell, M. P. (1971b). Parasite behaviour as a factor contributing to the stability of insect host-parasite interactions. Proc. Advan. Stud. Inst. Dynamics Numbers Popul., Oosterbeck, 1970, pp. 366–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behaviour, 2nd ed., McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1959a). The components of predation, as revealed by a study of small mammal predation of the European pine sawfly. Can. Entomol. 91: 293–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1959b). Some characteristics of simple types of predation. Can. Entomol. 91: 385–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1963). An experimental component analysis of population processes. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 32: 22–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1964). The analysis of complex population processes. Can. Entomol. 96: 335–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1965). The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 45:1–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulling, C. S. (1966). The functional response of invertebrate predators to prey density. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 48: 1–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, H. S. (1968). The adaptive significance of colonial nesting in the Brewer’s blackbird (Euphagus cyanocephalus). Ecology 49: 682–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, B. O., and Wood-Gush, D. C. M. (1971). A specific appetite for calcium in domestic chickens. Anim. Behay. 19: 490–499.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, F. W. (1958). An analysis of the concepts of discrimination and preference. Am. J. Psychol. 58: 152–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivlev, V. S. (1961). Experimental Ecology of the Feeding of Fishes, Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaddou, I. (1960). The feeding behaviour of Hippodamia quinquesignata (Kirby) larvae. Univ. Calif. Publ. Entomol. 16: 181–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kear, J. (1962). Food selection in finches with special reference to interspecific differences. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 138: 163–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R. (1971). Territory and breeding density in the great tit, Parus major L. Ecology 52: 2–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., MacRoberts, M. H., and Cullen, J. M. (1972). Flocking and feeding in the great tit: An experimental study. Ibis 114: 507–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, H. (1965). Parental feeding in the oystercatcher (Haematopus o. ostralegus (L.)). Dansk. Orn. Foren. Tidsskr. 59: 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landenberger, D. E. (1968). Studies on selective feeding in the starfish Pisaster in Southern California. Ecology 49: 1062–1075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H. (1972). Geographical Ecology, Harper and Row, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H., and Pianka, E. R. (1966). On the optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Naturalist 100: 603–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahmoud, I. Y., and Lavenda, N. (1969). Establishment and eradication of food preferences in red-eared turtles. Copeia, pp. 298–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manly, B. F. J, Miller. P., and Cook, L. M. (1972). Analysis of a selective predation experiment. Am. Naturalist 106: 719–736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menge, B. (1972). Foraging strategy of a starfish in relation to actual prey availability and environmental predictability. Ecol. Monogr. 42: 25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Messenger, P. S., and Force, D. C. (1963). An experimental host-parasite system: Therioaphis maculata (Buckton), Praon palitans Muesebeck (Homoptera: Aphidae, Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Ecology 44: 532–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mook, J. H., Mook, L. J., and Heikens, H. S. (1960). Further evidence for the role of “searching image” in the hunting behaviour of titmice. Arch. Néerl. Zool. 13: 448–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrell, G. M., and Turner, J. R. G. (1970). Experiments on mimicry. I. The response of wild birds to artificial prey. Behaviour 36: 116–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, H. C. (1971). Oddity and specific searching image more important than conspicuousness in prey selection. Nature (Land.) 233: 345–346.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murdoch, W. W. (1969). Switching in general predators: Experiments on predator specificity and stability of prey populations. Ecol. Monogr. 39: 335–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdoch, W. W. (1972). The functional response of predators. Proc. 14th Internat Entomol. Congr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murton, R. K. (1971). The significance of a specific search image in the feeding behaviour of the wood pigeon. Behaviour 40: 10–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neish, I. C. (1970). A comparative analysis of the feeding behaviour of two salamander populations in Marion Lake, B. C. Ph. D. thesis, University of British Columbia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton Griffiths, M. (1967). A study of the feeding behaviour of the oystercatcher Haematopus ostrelegus. D. Phil. thesis, Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H. (1969). Age and hunting success in the brown pelican (Pelecanus occidentalis). Anim. Behay. 17: 216–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H. (1971). Ecological aspects of behaviour. In Farner, D. S., and King, J. R. (eds.), Avian Biology, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paloheimo, J. (1971a). On a theory of search. Biometrika 58: 61–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paloheimo, J. (1971b). A stochastic theory of search: Implications for predator prey situations. Math. Biosci. 12: 105–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popham, E. J. (1941). The variation in colour of certain species of Arctocorisa (Hemiptera, Corixidae) and its significance. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. Ser. A 111: 135–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popham, E. J. (1942). Further experimental studies on the selective action of predators. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. Ser. A 112: 105–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitch, V. E. (1968). The role of experience in the development of food preferences in gull chicks. Anim. Behay. 16: 425–428.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitch, V. E. (1969). The role of experience in the development and retention of seed preferences in zebra finches. Behaviour 33: 222–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, D.J. (1971). An optimization model of food selection. Am. Naturalist 105: 575–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recher, H. F., and Recher, J. A. (1969). Comparative foraging efficiency of adult and immature little blue herons (Florida caerulea). Anim. Behay. 17: 320–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reighard, J. (1908). An experimental field study of warning coloration in coral reef fishes. Publ. Carnegie Inst. 103: 257–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, W. L. (1967). Specificity of specific hungers. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 64: 49–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royama, T. (1970). Factors governing the hunting behaviour and selection of food by the great tit (Parus major L.). J. Anim. Ecol. 39: 619–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royama, T. (1971). A comparative study of models for predation and parasitism. Res. Popul. Ecol. Kyoto Suppl. 1: 1–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salt, G. W. (1967). Predation in an experimental protozoan population (Woodruffia-Paramecium). Ecol. Monogr. 37: 113–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoener, T. W. (1969). Models of optimal size for solitary predators. Am. Naturalist 103: 277–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoener, T. W. (1971). Theory of feeding strategies. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2: 369–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (1972a). Constraints on learning. In Lehrman, D. S., Hinde, R. A., and Shaw, E. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 4, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (1972b). The role of novelty in learned avoidance of unpalatable “prey” by domestic chicks Gallus gallus. Anim. Behay. 20: 29–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, S., and Alcock, J. (1971). Learning and the foraging persistence of white-crowned sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys. Ibis 113: 477–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. C. (1970). The coevolution of pine squirrels (Tamiasciurus) and conifers. Ecol. Monogr. 40: 349–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. N. M. (1971). The searching behaviour and prey recognition of certain vertebrate predators. D. Phil. thesis, Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. N. M., and Dawkins, C. R. (1971). The hunting behaviour of individual great tits in relation to spatial variations in their food density. Anim. Behay. 19: 695–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Southwood, T. R. E. (1966). Ecological Methods, Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G., Budiansky, J., Spivak, J. G., and Johnson, M. C. (1971). Extremely rapid visual search: The maximum rate of scanning letters for the presence of a numeral. Science 174: 307–311.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, N. S. (1964) The learning of discriminations by animals. Endeavour 23: 148–152.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, N. S., and Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of Animal Discrimination Learning, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, L. (1949). De invloed van roofieren op de aantalssterkte van hun prooidieran. Vakblad. Biol. 28: 265–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, L. (1960). The natural control of insects in pine woods. I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. Arch. Néerl. Zool. 13: 265–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N., Impkoven, M., and Franck, D. (1967). An experiment on spacing out as a defence against predation. Behaviour 28: 307–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triesman, A. (1964). Selective attention in man. Brit. Med. Bull. 20: 12–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triesman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychol. Rev. 76: 382–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tullock, G. (1971). The coal tit as a careful shopper. Am. Naturalist 105: 77–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ware, D. M. (1971). Predation by rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri). The effect of experience. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 28: 1847–1852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willson, M. F. (1971). Seed selection in some North American finches. Condor 73: 415–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, H. G. (1965). Some factors that reduce the reproductive rate of Nasonia vitripermis (Walk.) at high population densities. Can. Entomol. 97: 970–977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1973 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Krebs, J.R. (1973). Behavioral Aspects of Predation. In: Bateson, P.P.G., Klopfer, P.H. (eds) Perspectives in Ethology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7569-6_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7569-6_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-7571-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-7569-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics