Skip to main content

Neuroscience, Gender, and “Development To” and “From”: The Example of Toy Preferences

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:

Abstract

“Development to” perspectives implicitly or explicitly assume that experience influences the individual’s development “to” a genetically encoded phenotype. By contrast, “development from” perspectives assume no genetically pre-specified developmental pathway, but the co-construction of the phenotype from the complex and dynamic interaction between environmental stimuli, genotype, and the organization of the nervous system at each developmental phase. This chapter examines the “brain organization” account of sex differences in toy preferences in light of challenges to the “development to” perspective, of which the brain organization account is an example. It is argued that there are significant methodological and conceptual issues, and empirical uncertainties, regarding each of four categories of evidence commonly cited as support for the brain organization account. The scientific and ethical need for research from a “development from” perspective for future investigations of this politically important and socially sensitive scientific question is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   999.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   999.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ah-King, M. (2009). Toy story: En vetenskaplig kritik av forskningom apors leksakspreferenser. Tidskrift för genusvetenskap, 1, 45–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue, and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32(1), 7–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G., & Saenz, J. (2012). Early androgens, activity levels and toy choices of children in the second year of life. Hormones and Behavior, 62, 500–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G., & Wilcox, T. (2012). Sex differences in early infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 400–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arthur, A., Bigler, R., Liben, L., Gelman, S., & Ruble, D. (2008). Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: A developmental intergroup perspective. In S. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 66–86). Oxford: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Chapman, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, S123–S130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behaviour in girls and in boys. Psychological Science, 20(2), 144–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Sex differences in mind: Keeping science distinct from social policy. In S. Ceci & W. Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 159–172). Washington, DC: APA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. SIGNS: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 8, 598–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J., Tennyson, R., & Wrangham, R. (2011). Male more than female infants imitate propulsive motion. Cognition, 121, 262–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S., & Bailey, J. (2003). Effects on gender identity of prenatal androgens and genital appearance: Evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 88(3), 1102–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3(3), 203–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S., & Resnick, S. (2007). The seeds of career choices: Prenatal sex hormone effects on psychology sex differences. In S. Ceci & C. Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 147–157). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R., & Liben, L. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bleier, R. (1986). Sex differences research: Science or belief? In R. Bleier (Ed.), Feminist approaches to science (pp. 147–164). New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breedlove, S., Cooke, B., & Jordan, C. (1999). The orthodox view of brain sexual differentiation. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 54, 8–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, F. D. (1972). The integration of biology and behavior in the socialization of Macaca sylvana of Gibraltar. In F. E. Poirier (Ed.), Primate Socialization. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, F. D. (1977). Ethology and the development of sex and gender identity in non-human primates. Acta Biotheoretica, 26(1), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, F. D. (1992). The social group as information unit: Cognitive behaviour, cultural processes. In F. D. Burton (Ed.), Social processes and mental abilities in non-human primates: Evidences from longitudinal field studies. Lewiston: Edwin Mellen Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G., Dickins, T., Sear, R., & Lalan, K. (2011). Evolutionary accounts of human behavioural diversity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 313–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106(4), 676–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 113–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, H. (2008). The role of values in expert reasoning. Public Affairs Quarterly, 22(1), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2013). The nature–nurture debates: 25 years of challenges in understanding the psychology of gender. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 340–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelmann, M. N., & Auger, A. P. (2011). Epigenetic impact of simulated maternal grooming on estrogen receptor alpha within the developing amygdala. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 25(7), 1299–1304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Escudero, P., Robbins, R. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Sex-related preferences for real and doll faces versus real and toy objects in young infants and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 367–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fausto-Sterling, A. (2005). The bare bones of sex: Part 1–Sex and gender. SIGNS: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 30(2), 1491–1527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fausto-Sterling, A., Coll, C., & Lamarre, M. (2012). Sexing the baby: Part 2 – Applying dynamic systems theory to the emergences of sex-related differences in infants and toddlers. Social Science & Medicine, 74, 1693–1702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, R. D. (2010). Change in the brain’s white matter. Science, 330(6005), 768–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C. (2010). Delusions of gender: How our minds, society, and neurosexism create difference. New York: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C. (2012). Explaining, or sustaining, the status quo? The potentially self-fulfilling effects of ‘hardwired’ accounts of sex differences. Neuroethics, 5(3), 285–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C., Jordan-Young, R. M., Kaiser, A., & Rippon, G. (2013). Plasticity, plasticity, plasticity … and the rigid problem of sex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 550–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gettler, L., McDade, T., Feranil, A., & Kuzawa, C. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(39), 13194–16199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gitau, R., Adams, D., Fisk, N., & Glover, V. (2005). Fetal plasma testosterone correlates positively with cortisol. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 90, F166–F169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1993). The pre-school activities inventory: A standardised assessment of gender role in children. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 131–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossi, G., & Fine, C. (2012). The role of fetal testosterone in the development of “the essential difference” between the sexes: Some essential issues. In R. Bluhm, A. Jacobson, & H. Maibom (Eds.), Neurofeminism: Issues at the intersection of feminist theory and cognitive neuroscience. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haier, R., Siegal, B., MacLachlan, A., Soderling, E., Lottenberg, S., & Buchsbaum, M. (1992). Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: A positron emission tomographic study. Brain Research, 570, 134–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, S., & McGarty, C. (2001). A 100 years of certitude? Social psychology, the experimental method and the management of scientific uncertainty. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54(3), 359–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, R. A., Measday, M. A., & Wallen, K. (2003). Sex differences in interest in infants in juvenile rhesus monkeys: Relationship to prenatal androgens. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 573–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Cell, 14(10), 448–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2011). Gender development and the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 69–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2013). There’s no good reason to push pink toys on girls. The Conversation. Retrieved 10 September 2013 from http://theconversation.com/theres-no-good-reason-to-push-pink-toys-on-girls-15830

  • Hines, M., & Alexander, G. (2008). Monkeys, girls, boys and toys: A confirmation. Letter regarding “Sex differences in toy preferences: Striking parallels between monkeys and humans”. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 478–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K., Golding, J., & Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team. (2002). Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: A longitudinal, population study. Child Development, 73(6), 1678–1687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M., Brook, C., & Conway, G. S. (2004). Androgen and pyschosexual development: Core gender identity, sexual orientation, and recalled childhood gender role behavior in women and men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Journal of Sex Research, 41, 75–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff Sommers, C. (2012). You can give a boy a doll, but you can’t make him play with it. The Atlantic. Retrieved 7 January 2013 from http://www.theatlantic.com/sexes/archive/2012/12/you-can-give-a-boy-a-doll-but-you-cant-make-him-play-with-it/265977/

  • Hyde, J. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itani, J. (1959). Paternal care in the wild Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata fuscata. Primates, 2(1), 61-93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jadva, V., Hines, M., & Golombok, S. (2010). Infants’ preferences for toys, colors, and shapes: Sex differences and similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(6), 1261–1273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jäncke, L., Gaab, N., Wüstenberg, T., Scheich, H., & Heinze, H. J. (2001). Short-term functional plasticity in the human auditory cortex: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 479–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan-Young, R. (2010). Brain storm: The flaws in the science of sex differences. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan-Young, R. (2012). Hormones, context, and “Brain Gender”: A review of evidence from congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Social Science & Medicine, 74(11), 1738–1744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, A. (2012). Re-conceptualizing “sex” and “gender” in the human brain. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 220(2), 130–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, A., Haller, S., Schmitz, S., & Nitsch, C. (2009). On sex/gender related similarities and differences in fMRI language research. Brain Research Reviews, 61(2), 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, G., & Rogers, L. (2003). Gene worship: Moving beyond the nature/nurture debate over genes, brain, and gender. New York: Other Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knickmeyer, R., Wheelwright, S., Taylor, K., Ragatt, P., Hackett, G., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Gender-typed play and amniotic testosterone. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, N. (2004). Understanding blindness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3, 315–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: The interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 171–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129(6), 819–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, M. V., Ashwin, E., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al. (2012). Fetal testosterone influences sexually dimorphic gray matter in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(2), 674–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(8), 4398–4403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W. A. (2002). The natural history of primate behavioral development: An organismic perspective. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (Eds.), Conceptions of development: Lessons from the laboratory. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, A. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 475–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer-Bahlburg, H., Dolezal, C., Zucker, K., Kessler, S., Schober, J., & New, M. (2006). The recalled childhood gender questionnaire-revised: A psychometric analysis in a sample of women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Sex Research, 43(4), 364–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mondragón-Ceballos, R., Chiappa, P., Mayagoitia, L., & Lee, P. (2010). Sex differences in learning the allocation of social grooming in infant stumptailed macaques. Behaviour, 147, 1073–1099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (1984). Maternal contributions to the development of masculine sexual behavior in laboratory rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 17(4), 347–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2002). On differences and development. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (Eds.), Conceptions of development: Lessons from the laboratory (pp. 57–76). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., Dou, H., & Juraska, J. (1992). Maternal stimulation affects the number of motor neurons in a sexually dimorphic nucleus of the lumbar spinal cord. Brain Research, 572, 52–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, M., Marlowe, F., Bugumba, R., & Ellison, P. (2009). Testosterone and paternal care in East African foragers and pastoralists. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 347–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nash, A., & Grossi, G. (2007). Picking Barbie’s brain: Inherent sex differences in scientific ability? Journal of Interdisciplinary Feminist Thought, 2(1), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordenström, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A., & Wedell, A. (2002). Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by CYP21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 87(11), 5119–5124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orenstein, P. (2011). Should the world of toys be gender-free? New York Times. Retrieved 7 January 2013 from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/12/30/opinion/does-stripping-gender-from-toys-really-make-sense.html?_r=0

  • Pasterski, V., Geffner, M., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76(1), 264–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phoenix, C. H., Goy, R. W., Gerall, A. A., & Young, W. C. (1959). Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology, 65(3), 369–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes, F. I., Winter, J. S. D., & Faiman, C. (1973). Studies on human sexual development. I. Fetal gonadal and adrenal sex steroids. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 37(1), 74–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodeck, C. H., Gill, D., Rosenberg, D. A., & Collins, W. P. (1985). Testosterone levels in midtrimester maternal and fetal plasma and amniotic fluid. Prenatal Diagnosis, 5, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saad, G. (2012). Sex-specific toy preferences: Learned or innate? Psychology Today. Retrieved 7 January 2013 from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/homo-consumericus/201212/sex-specific-toy-preferences-learned-or-innate

  • Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Nordenstrom, A., & Larsson, A. (2003). Prenatal androgens and gender-typed behavior: A study of girls with mild and severe forms of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 440–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Anders, S., & Watson, N. (2006). Social neuroendocrinology: Effects of social contexts and behaviors on sex steroids in humans. Human Nature, 17(2), 212–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van de Beek, C., Thijssen, J. H. H., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2004). Relationships between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord serum: What is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure? Hormones and Behavior, 46(5), 663–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van de Beek, C., Van Goozen, S., Buitelaar, J., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. (2009). Prenatal sex hormones (maternal and amniotic fluid) and gender-related play behavior in 13-month-old infants. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 6–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van de Waal, E., Renevy, N., Favre, C., & Bshary, R. (2010). Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, B, 277, 2105–2111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., & Thomas, M. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), 75–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W., Pasterski, V., Hindmarsh, P., Geffner, M., & Hines, M. (2012). Are there parental socialization effects on the sex-typed behavior of individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(3), 381–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Eagly, A. (2013). Biosocial construction of sex differences and similarities in behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 55–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cordelia Fine .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this entry

Cite this entry

Fine, C. (2015). Neuroscience, Gender, and “Development To” and “From”: The Example of Toy Preferences. In: Clausen, J., Levy, N. (eds) Handbook of Neuroethics. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_151

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_151

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-007-4706-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-007-4707-4

  • eBook Packages: Humanities, Social Sciences and Law

Publish with us

Policies and ethics