Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive accessibility and the capacity of cognitions to predict future depression: A theoretical note

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several recent studies have suggested that negative patterns of thinking are not predictive of future depression. Such conclusions are inconsistent with some current theories of depression (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1976; Ellis & Harper, 1975) that assign a major causal role to cognitions. In the present article some possible reasons for the null findings are discussed. First, it is suggested that individuals who are vulnerable to depression may differ from other persons by having highly negativistic thinking patterns, but that these patterns may be latent and less cognitively accessible than neutral or even positively valenced thinking patterns, except when it happens that the negativistic cognitions have been primed by situations (e.g., failure) analogous to the original situations in which they were learned. One important implication of these views concerns the type of testing situation in which responses to measures of an “insidious” cognitive style or negativistic thinking patterns will be most predictive of naturally occurring depression. Second, it is suggested that further attention needs to be given to certain measurement considerations that have proven important in the broader field of personality (e.g., Epstein, 1979).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87 49–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression. New York: Hober Medical.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Rush, A. J. (1981). Cognitive approaches to depression and suicide. In G. Serban (Ed.),Cognitive deficits in the development of mental illness. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior.Psychological Review, 81 506–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breslow, R., Kocsis, J., & Belkin, B. (1981). Contribution of the depressive perspective to memory function in depression.American Journal of Psychiatry, 138 227–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1964). Repression-sensitization as a dimension of personality. In B. A. Maher (Ed.),Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 87–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeMonbreun, B. G., & Craighead, W. E. (1977). Distortion of perception and recall of positive and neutral feedback in depression.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1 311–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A., & Harper, R. A (1975).A new guide to rational living. North Hollywood: Hal Leighton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 1197–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geller, V., & Shaver, P. (1976). The cognitive consequences of self-awareness.Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 12, 438–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Sweeney, P. D., & Shaeffer, D. E. (1981). The causality of causal attributions in depression: A crossed-lagged panel correlational analysis.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 14–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1980). Accessibility of social constructs: Information processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.),Cognition, social interaction, and personality. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13 141–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1979). Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1798–1809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Maladaptive cognitive structures in depression.American Journal of Psychiatry, 135 525–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J. L., Larson, D. W., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression-related cognitions: Antecedent or consequence?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 213–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, G. G., & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effects of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences.Psychological Medicine, 5, 173–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb, A., Beck, A. T., & Diggory, J. (1971). Differential effects of success and failure on depressed and nondepressed patients.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 152 106–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metalsky, G. I., Abramson, L. Y., Semmel, A., Seligman, M. E. P., & Peterson, C. (1980).Attributional style and the emotional response to negative events: A test of the reformulated model of learned helplessness and depression. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Stony Brook.

  • Miller, W. R. (1975). Psychological deficit in depression.Psychological Bulletin, 82 238–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Schwartz, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1981). Self-blame and depression symptoms.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 253–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L., & Brown, D. R. (1952). Percepted consequences of success and failure.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 213–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J. B., & Kriss, M. (1977). The cognitive dynamics of salience in the attribution process.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 49–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Rholes, W. S., & Eggers, J. (1982). The Velten Mood Induction Procedure: Effects on mood and memory.Journal of Consulting and Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50 146–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M. (1979). Remembering in and out of context.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5 460–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., Glenberg, A. M., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Environmental context and human memory.Memory and Cognition, 6 342–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M., & White, P. (1982). Mood and memories: Elation, depression, and the remembering of the events of one's life.Journal of Personality, 42, 221–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1980). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 841–856.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88 248–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Taylor, R. (1981) Induced mood and accessibility of memories: An effect of mood state or of inductio procedure?British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 39–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval process in episodic memory.Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability.Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, M., Segal, R., & Beck, A. T. (1974). An investigation of cognition and affect in normal men.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, I. M., & Blackburn, I. M. (1981). Cognitive style in depressed and recovered depressed patients.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20 283–292.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Order of authorship was determined by a random process. The authors assume equal responsibility for this paper. The authors wish to thank Emily Davidson, Jeff Kern, Charlene Muehlenhard, Jack Nation, Steve Smith, Dave Wilson, and anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier drift of this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Riskind, J.H., Rholes, W.S. Cognitive accessibility and the capacity of cognitions to predict future depression: A theoretical note. Cogn Ther Res 8, 1–12 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01315092

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01315092

Keywords

Navigation