Skip to main content
Log in

Genetic determinants of individual differences in avoidance learning: Behavioral and endocrine characteristics

  • Multi-Author Review
  • Published:
Experientia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Bidirectional genetic selection for good and poor active avoidance learning in a shuttle box has been carried out in three independent laboratories using remarkably similar discrete-trial training procedures. The resulting strains are known as the Roman High and Low Avoidance (RHA and RLA), the Syracuse High and Low Avoidance (SHA and SLA) and the Australian High and Low Avoidance (AHA and ALA) strains, respectively. An additional unidirectionally selected strain, known as the Tokai High Avoider (THA) strain was developed in Japan using a free-operant Sidman avoidance procedure in a Skinner box. This paper reviews the selection of the Syracuse strains, enumerates the various behavioral and endocrine characteristics of the strains, and compares them to the other similarly selected strains. The behavioral work suggests that genetic selection from diverse breeding stocks has resulted in common characteristics that differentiate the strains in the emotional, not learning, domain. The endocrine data, however, are somewhat at odds. The Syracuse strains differentiate one way with respect to endocrine function, and the Roman strains differentiate in the opposite way. We suggest, therefore, that the endocrine correlates are not tightly linked to the avoidance genotype. Genetic analysis of all of the selected strains for both the avoidance phenotype and the endocrine correlates will be needed to test this hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Anastasi, A., Differential Psychology (3rd ed.). Macmillan, New York, 1958.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Anisman, H., and Waller, T. G., Facilitative and disruptive effects of prior exposure to shock on subsequent avoidance performance. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.78 (1972) 113–122.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Archer J., Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: A review. Anim. Behav.21 (1973) 205–235.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bammer, G., Studies on two new strains of rats selectively bred for high or low conditioned avoidance responding. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Australian Society for the Study of Animal Behavior, Brisbane 1978.

  5. Bättig, K., Driscoll, P., Schlatter, J., and Uster, H. J., Effects of nicotine on the exploratory locomotion patterns of female Roman High- and Low-Avoidance rats. Pharmac. Biochem. Behav.4 (1976) 435–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bignami, G., Selection for high rates and low rates of avoidance conditioning in the rat. Anim. Behav.13 (1965) 221–227.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Berger, D. R., and Brush, F. R., Rapid acquisition of discrete-trial lever-press avoidance: Effects of signal-shock interval. J. exp. Analysis Behav.24 (1975) 227–239.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Bond, N. W., Avoidance, classical, and pseudoconditioning as a function of species-specific defense reaction in high- and low-avoider rat strains. Anim. Learn. Behav.12 (1984) 323–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Broadhurst, P. L., Determinants of emotionality in the rat. I. Situational factors. Br. J. Psychol.48 (1957) 1–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Broadhurst, P. L., The Maudsley reactive and nonreactive straisn of rats: A survey. Behav. Genet5 (1975) 299–319.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Broadhurst, P. L., and Bignami, G., Correlative effects of psychogenetic selection: A study of the Roman high and low avoidance strains of rats. Behav. Res. Ther.2 (1964) 273–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Brush, F. R., On the difference between animals that learn and do not learn to avoid electric shock. Psychon. Sci.5 (1966) 123–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Brush, F. R., The effects of amount of signaled escape training on subsequent avoidance learning. Psychon. Sci. 21 (1970) 51–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Brush, F. R., Genetic determinants of avoidance learning: Mediation by emotionality?, in: Affect, Conditioning, and Cognition: Essays on the Determinants of Behavior, pp. 27–42. Eds F. R. Brush and J. B. Overmier. Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, 1985.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Brush, F. R., Baron, S., Froehlich, J. C., Ison, J. R., Pellegrino, L. J., Phillips, D. S. Sakellaris, P. C., and Williams, V. N. Genetic differences in avoidance learning byRattus norvegicus. Escape/avoidance responding, sensitivity to electric shock, discrimination learning, and open-field behavior. J. comp. Psychol.99 (1985) 60–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Brush, F. R., Del Paine, S. N., Pellegrino, L. J., Rykaszewski, I. M., Dess, N. K., and Collins, P. Y., CER suppression, passive-avoidance learning, and stress-induced suppression of drinking in the Syracuse High- and Low-Avoidance strains of rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. comp. Psychol.102 (1988) 337–339.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Brush, F. R., Froehlich, J. C., and Sakellaris, P. C., Genetic selection for avoidance behavior in the rat. Behav. Genet.9 (1979) 309–316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Brush, F. R., Isaacson, M. D., Pellegrino, L. J., Rykaszewski, I. M., and Nagase Shain, C., Characteristics of the pituitary-adrenal system in the Syracuse high and low avoidance strains of rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Genet.21 (1991) 35–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Brush, F. R., and Nagase Shain, C., Endogenous opioids and behavior, in: Psychoendocrinology, pp. 379–435. Eds. F. R. Brush and S. Levine. Academic Press, San Diego 1989.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  20. DeFries, J. C., Gervais, M. C., and Thomas, A. E., Response to 30 generations of selection for open-field activity in laboratory mice. Behav. Genet.8 (1978) 3–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Denenberg, V. H., Open-field behavior in the rat: What does it mean? Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.159 (1969) 852–859.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Driscoll, P., Genetic models in brain and behavior research. Part 1. Introduction. Experientia44 (1988) 463–464.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Driscoll, P., and Bättig, K., Behavioral, emotional and neurochemical profiles of rats selected for extreme differences in active, two-way avoidance performance, in: Genetics of the Brain, pp. 95–123. Ed. I. Liebich. Elsevier Biomedical Press, Amsterdam 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Driscoll, P., Woodson, P., Fuemm, H., and Bättig K., Selection for two-way avoidance deficit inhibits shock-induced fighting in the rat. Physiol. Behav.24 (1980) 793–795.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Falconer, D. S., Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Oliver and Boyd, London 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Ferster C. B., and Skinner, B. F., Schedules of Reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York 1957.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  27. File, S. E., and Vellucci, S. V., Behavioural and biochemical measures of stress in hooded rats from different sources. Physiol. Behav.22 (1979) 31–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Fisher, R. A., The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Clarendon Press, Oxford 1930.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  29. Fisher, R. A., The Theory of Inbreeding. Academic Press, New York 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Flaherty, C. R., and Rowan, G. A., Rats (Rattus norvegicus) selectively bred to differ in avoidance behavior also differ in response to novelty stress, in glycemic conditioning, and in reward contrast. Behav. neural Biol.51 (1989) 145–164.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Fulker, D. W., Wilcock, J., and Broadhurst, P. L., Studies in genotype-environment interaction. I. Methodology and preliminary multivariate analysis of a diallel cross of eight strains of rat. Behav. Genet.2 (1972) 261–287.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Fuller, J. L., and Simmel, E. C., Trends in behavior genetics, 1960–1985, in: Perspective in Behavior Genetics, pp. 1–27. Eds J. L. Fuller and E. C. Simmel. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Fuller, J. L., and W. R. Thompson, Behavior Genetics. Wiley, New York 1960.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  34. Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., and Feer, H., Locomotor activity, defecation score and corticosterone levels during an openfield exposure: A comparison among individually and group-housed rats, and genetically selected rat lines. Physiol. Behav.27 (1981) 183–186.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Driscoll, P., and Feer, H., Differential hormonal physiological responses to stress in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Physiol. Behav.28 (1982) 259–263.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Govind, C. K., Asymmetry in lobster claws. Am. Sci.77 (1989) 468–474.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Gray, J. A., and Lalljee, B., Sex differences in emotional behavior in the rat: Correlations between open-field defecation and active avoidance. Anim. Behav.22 (1974) 856–861.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Guenaire, C., and Delacour J., Two-way avoidance behavior of the Roman rat strains following brain lesions. Physiol. Behav.31 (1983) 159–166.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Guenaire, C., and Delacour, J., Differential acquisition of a ‘working memory’ task by the Roman strains of rats. Physiol. Behav.34 (1985) 705–707.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Hall, C. S., The inheritance of emotionality. Sigma Xi Quarterly26 (1938) 17–27.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Hirsch, J., Ed., Behavior-Genetic Analysis. McGraw Hill, New York 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Hirsch, J., To ‘unfrock the charlatans’. Sage Race Relations Abstr.6 (1981) 1–67.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Hull, C. L., Principles of Behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Imaaa, H., Emotional reactivity and conditionability in four strains of rats. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.79 (1972) 474–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Jensen, A. R., Statement of Dr. Arthur R. Jensen before the General Subcomittee on Education, House Education and Labor Committee. Congressional Record, I-237, Washington, DC, 1970.

  46. Joffe, J., Avoidance learning and failure to learn in two strains of rats selectively bred for emotionality. Psychon. Sci.1 (1964) 185–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Joffe, J., Genotype and prenatal and premating stress interact to affect adult behavior in rats. Science150 (1965) 1844–1855.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Kallmann, F. J., The genetic theory of schizophrenia: An analysis of 691 schizophrenic twin index families. Am. J. Psychiat.103 (1946) 309–322.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Kamin, L. J., Retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.50 (1957) 457–460.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Kamin, L. J., Retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response: Some further analyses. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.56 (1963) 713–718.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kamin, L. J., The Science and Politics of I. Q. Erlbaum, Potomac, MD, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Katzev, R. D., and Mills, S. K., Strain differences in avoidance conditioning as a function of the classical CS-US contingency. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.87 (1974) 661–671.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Levine, S., and Broadhurst, P. L., Genetic and ontogenetic determinants of adult behavior in the rat. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.56 (1963) 423–428.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Levine, S., and Wetzel, A., Infantile experiences, strain differences and avoidance conditioning. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.56 (1963) 879–881.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Martin, J. R., and Bättig, K., Acquisition and extinction of gustatory aversion in two lines of rats selectively bred for differential shuttlebox avoidance performance. Behav. Processes5 (1980) 303–310.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Martin, J. R., Overstreet, D. H., Driscoll, P., and Bättig, K., Effects of scopolamine, pilocarpine, and oxotremorine on the exploratory behavior of two psychogenetically selected lines of rats in a complex maze. Psychopharmacology72 (1981) 135–142.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Meyer, D. R., Cho, C., and Wesemann, A. F., On problems of conditioned discriminated lever-press avoidance responses. Psychol. Rev.67 (1960) 224–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Nagase, C. S., Randich, A., and Brush, F. R., Genetic differences in avoidance behavior: Cardiovascular activity, pain sensitivity and stress-induced analgesia. Peptides6, suppl. 1 (1985) 29–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Nagase, C. S., and Brush, F. R., Genetic differences in avoidance learning covary with non-opioid stress-induced analgesia. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.467 (1986) 416–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Okamoto, K., and Aoki, K., Development of a strain of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Jap. Circulation J.27 (1963) 282–293.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Overstreet, D. H., Russell, R. W., Helps, S. C., and Messenger, M., Selective breeding for sensitivity to the anticholinesterase DFP. Psychopharmacology65 (1979) 15–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Parsons, P. A., The Genetic Analysis of Behaviour. Methuen & Co., London 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Pavlov, I. P., Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford University Press, London 1927.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Pavlov, I. P., Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes. Liveright, New York 1928.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Pavlov, I. P., Conditioned Reflexes and Psychiatry. International, New York 1941.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. A., and McClearn, G. E., Behavioral Genetics: A Primer. W. H. Freeman, San Francisco 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Satinder, K. P., Genotype-dependent effects of d-amphetamine sulphate and caffeine on escape-avoidance behavior of rats. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.76 (1971) 359–364.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Satinder, K. P., Sensory responsiveness and avoidance learning in rats. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.90 (1976) 946–957

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Satinder, K. P., Arousal explains difference in avoidance learning of genetically selected rat strains. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.91 (1977) 1326–1336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Satinder, K. P., Genetically heterogeneous and selected lines of rats: Behavioral and reproductive comparison. Behav. Genet.10 (1980) 191–200.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Satinder, K. P., and Hill, K. D., Effects of genotype and postnatal experience on activity, avoidance, shock threshold, and open-field behavior of rats. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.86 (1974) 363–374.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Schönemann, P. H., New questions about old heritability estimates. Bul. psychon. Soc.27 (1989) 175–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Shigeta, S., Misawa, T., Yoshida, T., Aikawa, H., Momotani, H., and Miyake, T., Neurobehavioral analysis of high-rate Sidman avoidance rat strain. Jap. J. Psychopharmac.9 (1989) 217–224.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Shigeta, S., Miyake, K. Misawa, T., Aikawa, H., Yoshida, T., and Katoh, H., A new inbred rat strain ‘THA’. Rat News Letter, No.23 (1990) 9–11.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Singh, P. J., Sakellaris, P. C., and Brush, F. R., Retention of active and passive avoidance responses tested in extinction. Learning Motivation2 (1971) 305–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Skinner, B. F., Upon Further Reflection. Prentice-Hall, Engelwood Cliffs, NJ, 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Spence, K. W., Behavior Theory and Conditioning. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT, 1956.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  78. Suarez, S. D., and Gallup, G. G. Jr, An ethological analysis of openfield behavior in rats and mice. Learning Motivation12 (1981) 342–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Tryon, R. C., Genetic differences in maze learning ability in rats. Yb. natl. Soc. Study Education39 (1940) 111–119.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Valtin, H., and Schroeder, H. A., Familial hypothalamic diabetes insipidus in rats (Brattleboro strain). Am. J. Physiol.206 (1964) 425–430.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Walker, C.-D., Rivest, R. W., Meaney, M. J., and Aubert, M. L., Differential activation of the pituitary-adrenocortical axis after stress in the rat: Use of two genetically selected lines (Roman Low-and High-Avoidance rats) as a model. J. Endocr.123 (1989) 477–485.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Wilcock, J., and Fulker, D. W., Avoidance learning in rats: Genetic evidence for two distinct behavioral processes in the shuttlebox. J. comp. physiol. Psychol.82 (1973) 247–253.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Wilder, J., Stimulus and Response: The Law of Initial Value. John Wright and Sons, Bristol 1967.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Preparation of this paper was supported by research grant MH-39230-3 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brush, F.R. Genetic determinants of individual differences in avoidance learning: Behavioral and endocrine characteristics. Experientia 47, 1039–1050 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01923339

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01923339

Key words

Navigation