Skip to main content
Log in

Visual displays and contextual presentations in computer-based instruction

  • Research
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effects of two instructional strategies, visual display and contextual presentation, were investigated in the acquisition of electronic troubleshooting skills using computer-based instruction. Three types of visual displays (animation, static graphics with motion cues, and static graphics without motion cues) were used to represent structures and functions of electronic circuits and trouble-shooting procedures. The first hypothesis was that animation would be more effective than static graphics, but that static graphics with adequate motional cues representing the dynamic aspects of the task would accomplish results similar to animation. Results supported this hypothesis. The second hypothesis was that context-dependent instruction would be more efficient than context-independent instruction for solving problems in similar contexts, but that context independent instruction would be more effective in solving problems encountered in different contexts. The results supported this hypothesis. A general conclusion of this study is that the use of visual displays and contextual presentation should be based on the learning requirements of the task and the expected roles of the strategy in the learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J., Reder, L., & Simon, H. (1996). Situated learning and education.Educational Researcher, 25, 5–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ash, D.W. (1988). Part versus whole learning: Task separation and reintegration strategies.Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baek, Y.K., & Layne, B.H. (1988). Color, graphics, and animation in computer-assisted learning tutorial lesson.Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 15, 131–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., & Holyoak, K.J. (1989). Transfer of domain-specific problem solving procedures.Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 522–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R.A., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1989). On the puzzling relationship between environment context and human memory. In C. Izawa (Ed.),Current issues in cognitive processes: The Tulane Flowerree Symposium on Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1994). The advancement of learning.Educational Researcher, 23, 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L., & Campione, J.C. (1994). Guided discovery in a community of learners. In K. McGilly (Ed.),Classroom lesson: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 229–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.S., & Palincsar, A.S. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.),Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 393–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, J., & Hannafin, M. (1995). Situated cognition and learning environments: Roles, structures, and implications for design.Educational Technology Research and Development, 43, 53–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cognition & Technology Group at Vanderbilt, (1990). Anchored instruction and its relationships to situated cognition.Educational Research, 19, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cognition & Technology Group at Vanderbilt, (1993). Anchored instruction and situated cognition revisited.Educational Technology, 33, 52–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A.S., Brown, J.S., & Newman, S.E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.),Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 453–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A.A. (1982). Unlearning Aristotelian physics: A study of knowledge-based learning.Cognitive Science, 6, 37–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E. (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 764–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elio, R. (1986). Representation of similar well-learned cognitive procedures.Cognitive Science, 10, 41–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, F.N. (1924).Visual education: A comparative study of motion picture and other methods of instruction. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Stevens, A.L. (1983),Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J.G. (1989). Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.),Complex information processing (pp. 285–318. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983).Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, M.K., Proffitt, D.R., & Anderson, K. (1985). Judgments of natural and anomalous trajectories in the presence and absence of motion.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 795–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D.E. (1988). What mental model should be taught: Choosing instructional content for complex engineering systems. In J. Psotka, L.D. Massey, & S.A. Mutter (Eds.),Intelligent tutoring systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D.E., & Bovair, S. (1984). The role of a mental model in learning to operate a device.Cognitive Science, 8, 255–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Carver, S.M. (1988). Cognitive objectives in a LOGO debugging curriculum: Instruction, learning and transfer.Cognitive Psychology, 20, 362–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knerr, C.M., Morrison, J.E., Mumaw, R.J., Stein, D.J., Sticha, P.J., Hoffman, R.G., Buede, D.M., & Holding, D.M. (1987). Simulation-based research in part-task training. (Technical Report No.AF HRL-TR-86-12). Brooks Air Force Base, TX: Air Force Human Resources Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S.M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, M.A.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991).Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J., & Lesgold, A. (1978). On pictures in prose.Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 26, 233–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mane, A.M., Adams, J.A., & Donchin, E. (1989). Adaptive and part-whole training in the acquisition of a complex perceptual-motor skill.Acta Psychologica, 71, 179–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. (1997). Multimedia learning: Are we asking the right question?Educational Psychologist, 32, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., & Anderson, R. (1991). Animations need narrations: An experimental test of a dual-coding hypothesis.Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 484–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., & Anderson, R. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 444–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., Bover, W., Bryman, A., Mars, R., & Tapangco, L. (1996). When less is more: Meaningful learning from visual and verbal summaries of science textbook lessons.Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 64–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., & Gallini, J. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words?Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., & Sims, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extension of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., Steinhoff, K., Bower, G., & Mars, R. (1995). A generative theory of textbook design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text.Educational Technology Research and Development, 43, 31–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naylor, J.C. (1962). Parameters affecting the relative efficiency of part and whole training methods: A review of literature(Report No. NA VTRADEVCEN 950-1). Port Washington, NY: U. S. Naval Training Device Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naylor, J.C., & Briggs, G.E. (1963). Effects of task complexity and task organization on the relative efficiency or part and whole training methods.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K.M., Carlton, M.J., Fisher, A.T., & Rutter, B.G. (1989). Whole-part training strategies for learning the response dynamics of microprocessor driven simulators.ActaPsychologica, 71, 197–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. (1983). Some observations on mental models. In D. Gentner & A.L. Stevens (Eds.),Mental Models. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Publishers, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, O., & Gittelman, S. (1992). Selective use of animation and feedback in computer-based instruction.Educational Technology Research and Development, 40, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, O., & Gittelman, S. (1995). Dynamic characteristics of mental models and dynamic visual displays.Instructional Science, 23, 303–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, O., & Hopkins, R. (1993). Instructional conditions for using dynamic visual displays: A review.Instructional Science, 22, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, J. (1992).Training: Research andpractice. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, H., & Dalker, K.C. (1982). Graphics and animation as instructional tools: A case study.Pipeline, 7, 11–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reder, L.M., & Ritter, F. (1992). What determines initial feeling of knowledge? Familiarity with question terms, not with the answer.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 435–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S.K. (1985). Effects of computer graphics on improving estimates to algebra word problems.Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 285–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L.P. (1989). The effects of computer animated elaboration strategies and practice on factual and application learning in an elementary science lesson.Journal of Educational Computing Research, 5, 431–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiber, L.P. (1990a). Using computer animated graphics in science instruction with children.Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 135–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiber, L.P. (1990b). Animation in computer-based instruction.Educational Technology Research and Development, 38, 77–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiber, L.P., Boyce, M.J., & Assad, C. (1990). The effects of computer animation on adult learning and retrieval tasks.Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 17, 46–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiber, L.P., & Hannafin, M.J. (1988). The effects of textual and animated orienting activities and practice on learning from computer-based instruction.Computers in the Schools, 5, 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reif, F. (1987). Instructional design, cognition and technology: Applications to the teaching of scientific concepts.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24, 309–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rigney, J.W., & Lutz, K.A. (1976). Effects of graphic analogies of concepts in chemistry on learning and attitude.Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 305–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A.H. (1985).Mathematicalproblem solving. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M.K., & Anderson, J.R. (1989).Transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R.J., Feltovich, P.J., Jacobson, M.J., & Coulson, R.S. (1991). Knowledge representation, content specification, and the development of skill in situation specific knowledge assembly: Some constructivist issues as they relate to cognitive flexibility theory and hypertext.Educational Technology, 31, 22–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tessmer, M., & Richey, R.C. (1997). The role of context in learning and instructional design.Educational Technology Research and Development, 45, 85–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, B.Y. (1984). Designing computer games to help physics students understand Newton's laws of motion.Cognition and instruction, 1, 69–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, B.Y., & Frederiksen, J.R. (1990). Causal model progressions as a foundation for intelligent learning environments.Artificial intelligence, 42, 99–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wightman, D.C., & Sistrunk, F. (1987). Part-task training strategies in simulated carrier landing final-approach training.Human Factors, 29, 245–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolf, B., Blegan, D., Jansen, J., & Verloop, A. (1986).Teaching a complex industrial process. Philadelphia, PA: National Association of Artificial Intelligence.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1912).An introduction to psychology. London: G. Allen and Company.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

When this study was conducted, he was with the U.S. Army Research Institute. The opinions expressed herein are the author's and do not express or imply the views of the U.S. Department of Education or the U.S. Army Research Institute.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Park, Oc. Visual displays and contextual presentations in computer-based instruction. ETR&D 46, 37–50 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02299760

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02299760

Keywords

Navigation