Skip to main content
Log in

Toward a design theory of problem solving

  • Development
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Problem solving is generally regarded as the most important cognitive activity in everyday and professional contexts. Most people are required to and rewarded for solving problems. However, learning to solve problems is too seldom required in formal educational settings, in part, because our understanding of its processes is limited. Instructional-design research and theory has devoted too little attention to the study of problem-solving processes. In this article, I describe differences among problems in terms of their structuredness, domain specificity (abstractness), and complexity. Then, I briefly describe a variety of individual differences (factors internal to the problem solver) that affect problem solving. Finally, I articulate a typology of problems, each type of which engages different cognitive, affective, and conative processes and therefore necessitates different instructional support. The purpose of this paper is to propose a metatheory of problem solving in order to initiate dialogue and research rather than offering a definitive answer regarding its processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J.R. (1980).Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrows, H.S. (1985).How to design a problem-based curriculum for the pre-clinical years. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrows, H.S., & Tamblyn, R.M. (1980).Problem-based learning: An approach to medical education. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J., & Stein, B.S. (1984).The IDEAL problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity. New York: W.H. Freeman. Bryson, M., Bereiter, C., Scardamalia, M., & Joram, E. (1991). Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers. In R.J. Sternberg & P.A. Frensch (Eds.),Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 61–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, J.E., & Sternberg, R.J. (1998). Smart problem solving: How metacognition helps. In D.J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A.C. Graesser (Eds.),Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 47–68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J.K., & Haueisen, W.C. (1976). Field independence and hypothesis testing.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 763–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D., & Wearing, A.J. (1995). Complex problem solving: Toward a theory. In P.A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.),Complex problem solving: The European perspective (pp. 65–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunkle, M.E., Schraw, G., & Bendixen, L.D. (1995, April).Cognitive processes in well-defined and ill-defined problem solving. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.

  • Durso, F.T., & Gronlund, S.D. (1999). Situation awareness. In F.T. Durso (Ed.),Handbook of applied cognition (pp. 283–314). Chichester, UK: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • English, L.D. (1998). Children's reasoning in solving relational problems of deduction.Thinking & Reasoning, 4(3), 249–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, D.D., Eckart, T., Lauver, E., van Leeuwen, R., & Langemeyer, D. (1990). Learners' questions and comprehension in a tutoring system.Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 163–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and comprehension monitoring: A new era of cognitive development inquiry.American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J.P. (1982). Reactions to life dilemmas: Risk taking, success and responsibility attribution.Australian Journal of Psychology, 34, 25–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funke, J. (1991). Solving complex problems: Exploration and control of complex systems. In R.J. Sternberg & P.A. Frensch (eds.),Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 185–222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R.M. (1980).The conditions of learning. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R.M., Briggs, L.J., & Wager, W.W. (1992).Principles of instructional design (4th Ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace, & Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R.M., & Merrill, M.D. (1990). Integrative goals for instructional design.Educational Technology Research & Development, 38(1), 23–30 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M.L. (1986). Problem-solving strategies.Educational Psychologist, 21(1&2), 99–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M.L., & Holyoak, K.J. (1980). Analogical problem solving.Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M.L., & Holyoak, K.J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer.Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., & Pirolli, P. (1989). Motivating the notion of generic design within information processing theory: The design problem space.AI Magazine, 10(1), 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S.E., & Gill, R.T. (1989).The formation and use of knowledge structures in problem solving domains. Tech. Report AFOSR-88-0063. Washington, DC: Bolling AFB.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gourgey, A.F. (1998). Metacognition and basic skills instruction.Instructional Science, 26, 81–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. (1978). Natures of problem-solving abilities. In W. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (pp. 239–270). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. (1991). A view of mathematical problem solving in school. In M.U. Smith (Ed.),Toward a unified theory of problem solving (pp. 69–98). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guindon, R. (1990). Designing the design process: Exploiting opportunistic thoughts.Human-Computer Interaction, 5, 305–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halgren, S.L., & Cooke, N.J. (1993). Towards ecological validity in menu research.International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 39(1), 51–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E.P., Gott, S.P., & Pokorny, R.A. (1995).A procedural guide to cognitive task analysis: The PARI methodology, Tech. Report AL/HR-TR-1995-0108. Brooks Air Force Base, TX: Human Resources Directorate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannafin, M.J., Hall, C., Land, S., & Hill, J. (1994). Learning in open-ended learning environments: Assumptions, methods, and implications.Educational Technology, 34(8), 48–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J.R., & Simon, H.A. (1977). Psychological differences among problem isomorphs. In N.J. Castellan, D.B. Pisoni, & G.R. Potts (Eds.),Cognitive theory (pp. 21–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heil, M.C. (1999).Air traffic control specialist age and cognitive test performance. FAA Office of Aviation Medicine Report No. DOT-FAA-AM-99-23. Oklahoma City, OK: Federal Aviation Administration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, L.C. (1982). An exploration of the effect of structure variables on mathematical word problem-solving achievement (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University),Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, B.K., & Pintrich, P.R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning.Review of Educational Research, 67(1), 88–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, N.S., Jonassen, D.H., & McGee, S. (in press). Predictors of well-structured and ill-structured problem solving in an astronomy simulation.Journal of Research in Science Teaching.

  • Jeffries, R., Turner, A.A., Polson, P.G., & Atwood, M.E. (1981). The processes involved in designing software. In J.R. Anderson (Ed.),Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 255–283). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S.D. (1988). Cognitive analysis of expert and novice troubleshooting performance.Performance Improvement Quarterly, 1(3), 38–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S.D., & Satchwell, S.E. (1993). The effect of functional flow diagrams on apprentice aircraft mechanics' technical system understanding.Performance Improvement Quarterly, 6(4), 73–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H. (1997). Instructional design model for well-structured and ill-structured problem-solving learning outcomes.Educational Technology Research and Development 45(1), 65–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H. (2000a). Integrating problem solving into instructional design. In R.A. Reiser & J. Dempsey (Eds.),Trends and issues in instructional design and technology. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H. (2000b). Activity theory revisited. In D.H Jonassen & S.L. Land, (Eds.),Theoretical foundations of learning environments. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H. (2000c).Using technologies to model student problem spaces. Paper presented at the International Conference on Computers in Education, Taipei, Taiwan.

  • Jonassen, D.H., Beissner, K., & Yacci, M. (1993).Structural knowledge: Techniques for assessing, conveying, and acquiring structural knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H., & Grabowski, B.L. (1993).Handbook of individual differences, learning and instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H., & Henning, P. (1999). Mental models: Knowledge in the head and knowledge in the world.Educational Technology, 39(3), 37–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H., & Kwon, H.I. (in press). Communication patterns in computer-mediated vs. face-to-face group problem solving.Educational Technology Research and Development.

  • Jonassen, D.H, & Land, S.L (Eds.). (2000).Theoretical foundations of learning environments. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D., Prevish, T., Christy, D., Stavurlaki, E. (1999). Learning to solve problems on the Web: Aggregate planning in a business management course.Distance Education: An International Journal, 20(1), 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H., & Tessmer, M. (1996/1997). An outcomes-based taxonomy for instructional systems design, evaluation, and research.Training Research Journal, 2, 11–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D.H., Tessmer, M., & Hannum, W. (1999).Handbook of task analysis procedures. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahney, H. (1993).Problem solving: Current issues. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerstholt, J.H., & Raaijmakers, J.G.W. (1997). Decision making in dynamic task environments. In R. Ranyard, W.R. Cozier, & Ola Swenson (Eds.),Decision making: Cognitive models and explanations (pp. 205–217). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchner, K.S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition, and epistemic cognition: A three-level model of cognitive processing.Human Development, 26, 222–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluwe, R.H. (1995). Single case studies and models of complex problem solving In P.A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.),Complex problem solving: The European perspective (pp. 269–291). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Land, S.M., & Hannafin, M.J. (1996). A conceptual framework for the development of theories-inaction with open-ended learning environments.Educational Technology Research & Development, 44(3), 37–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, D., Lempert, R., & Nisbett, R.E. (1988). The effects of graduate training on reasoning: Formal discipline and thinking about everyday-life events.Educational Psychologist, 43, 431–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester, F.K. (1994). Musings about mathematical problem-solving research: 1970–1994.Journal for Research in Mathematis Education, 25, 660–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucangelli, D., Tressoldi, P.E., & Cendron, M. (1998). Cognitive and metacognitive abilities involved in the solution of mathematical word problems: Validation of a comprehensive model.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 257–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, E., & O'Neill, P. (1992). What creaes the dilemma in ethical dilemmas? Examples from psychological practice.Ethics & Behavior, 2(4), 227–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, T.J. (1981). The relation between field-independence and rule-transfer (Doctoral dissertation, University of Toledo),Dissertation Abstracts International, 442, 2575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, S.P. (1995).Schemas in problem solving. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masui, C., & DeCorte, E. (1999). Enhancing learning and problem solving skills: Orienting and self-judging, two powerful and trainable learning skills.Learning and Instruction, 9, 517–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1992).Thinking, problem solving, cognition (2nd ed.). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1998). Cognitive. Metacognitive, and motivational aspects of problem solving.Instructional Science, 26, 49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Wittrock, M.C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. In D.C. Berlinert & R.C. Calfee (Eds.),Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 47–62). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., Caramaza, A., & Basili, A. (1985). Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia.Brain and Cognition, 4, 171–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meacham, J.A., Emont, N.C. (1989). The interpersonal basis of everyday problem solving. In J.D. Sinnott (Ed.),Everyday problem solving: Theory and applications (pp. 7–23). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, J.D., & Roth, B.M. (1991).Decision making: Its logic and practice. Savage, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972).Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. (1965).The logic of collective action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D.N., Hancock, C., Hobbs, R., Martin, F., & Simmons, R. (1986). Conditions of learning in novice programmers.Journal of Educational Computing Research, 2(1), 37–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, W.G. (1970).The forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years: A scheme. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pokorny, R.A., Hall, E.P., Gallaway, M.A., & Dibble, E. (1996). Analyzing components of work samples to evaluate performance.Military Psychology, 8(3), 161–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S.K., Ernsyt, G.W., & Banerji, R. (1974). The role of analogy in transfer between similar problem states.Cognitive Psychology, 6, 436–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reigeluth, C.M. (1983).Instructional design theories and models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitman, W.R. (1965).Cognition and thought. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, W.C. (1990). Detection of cognitive structure with protocol data: Predicting performance on physics transfer problems.Cognitive Science, 14, 253–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronning, McCurdy, & Ballinger (1984, January). Individual differences: A third component in problem-solving instruction.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 21(1), 71–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D.N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon.Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, J., Chung, G.K.W.K., & Dorr, A. (1998). Children's Internet searching on complex problems: Performance and process analyses.Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 49, 840–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R.C., Fano, A., Bell, B., & Jona, M. (1993/1994). The design of goal-based scenarios.The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(4), 305–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, D.A. (1995). An introduction to social dilemmas. In D.A. Schroeder (Ed.),Social dilemmas: Perspectives on individuals and groups (pp. 1–14). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shavelson, R.J. (1972). Some aspects of the correspondence between content structure and cognitive structure in physics instruction.Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 225–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrill, J.M. (1983). Solving textbook mathematical problems.Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 29, 140–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. (1973). The structure of ill-structured problems.Artificial Intelligence, 4, 181–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, D.P. (1978). Information processing theory of human problem solving. In D. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M.K., & Anderson, J.R. (1989).The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M.U. (1991). A view from biology. In M.U. Smith (ed.),Toward a unified theory of problem solving. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P.L., & Ragan, T.J. (1999).Instructional design 2nd ed. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J., & Frensch, P.A. (Eds.). (1991).Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewin, L., & Anderson, C. (1974). Cognitive complexity as a determinant of information processing.Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 20(3), 233–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suedfeld, P., de Vries, B., Bluck, S., Wallbaum, B.C. (1996). Intuitive perceptions of decision-making strategy: Naive assessors' concepts of integrative complexity.International Journal of Psychology, 31(5), 177–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.Science, 211, 453–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1997).Training complex cognitive skills. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss, J.F., & Post, T.A. (1988). On the solving of ill-structured problems. In M.T.H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M.J. Farr (Eds.),The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss, J.F., Wolfe, C.R., Lawrence, J.A., & Engle, J.A. (1991). From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations. In R.J. Sternberg & P.A. Frensch (Eds.),Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 119–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. (1991). Managerial problem solving. In R.J. Sternberg & P.A. Frensch (Eds.),Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 159–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, S. (1992). Putting case-based instruction into context: Examples for legal and medical education.The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(4), 367–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, P.K. (1983). Inquiring systems and problem structures: Implications for cognitive development.Human Development, 26, 249–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D.R., Hrymak, A.N., Marshall, R.R., Wood, P.E., Crowe, Hoffman, T.W., Wright, J.D., Taylor, P.A., Woodhouse, K.A., & Bouchard, C.G.K. (1997). Developing problem-solving skills: the McMaster problem solving program.Journal of Engineering Education, 86(2), 75–92.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This paper represents an effort to introduce issues and concerns related to problem solving to the instructional design community. I do not presume that the community is ignorant of problem solving or its literature, only that too little effort has been expended by the field in articulating design models for problem solving. There are many reasons for that state of affairs.

The curse of any introductory paper is the lack of depth in the treatment of these issues. To explicate each of the issues raised in this paper would require a book (which is forthcoming), which makes it unpublishable in a journal. My purpose here is to introduce these issues in order to stimulate discussion, research, and development of problem-solving instruction that will help us to articulate better design models.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jonassen, D.H. Toward a design theory of problem solving. ETR&D 48, 63–85 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02300500

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02300500

Keywords

Navigation