Skip to main content
Log in

Instructional design for advanced learners: Establishing connections between the theoretical frameworks of cognitive load and deliberate practice

  • Special Issue
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive load theory (CLT) has been successful in identifying instructional formats that are more effective and efficient than conventional problem solving in the initial, novice phase of skill acquisition. However, recent findings regarding the “expertise reversal effect” have begun to stimulate cognitive load theorists to broaden their horizon to the question of how instructional design should be altered as a learner's knowledge increases. To answer this question, it is important to understand how expertise is acquired and what fosters its development. Expert performance research, and, in particular, the theoretical framework of deliberate practice have given us a better understanding of the principles and activities that are essential in order to excel in a domain. This article explores how these activities and principles can be used to design instructional formats based on CLT for higher levels of skills mastery. The value of these formats for e-learning environments in which learning tasks can be adaptively selected on the basis of online assessments of the learner's level of expertise is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R.M.J.P., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2001). Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency.Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 575–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction.Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, N., Reingold, E.M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D.M. (2001). The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements.Memory & Cognition, 29, 1146–1152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.), (1988).The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deakin, J.M., & Cobley, S. (2003). A search for deliberate practice: An examination of the practice environment in figure skating and volleyball. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 115–132). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A. (2002). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 21–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A. (2003). How the expert-performance approach differs from traditional approaches to expertise in sports: In search of a shared theoretical framework for studying expert performance. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 371–401). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory.Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance.Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., & Lehmann, A.C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence for maximal adaptation to task constraints.Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., & Simon, H.A. (1993).Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (Rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P.M., & Posner, M.I. (1967).Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P., & Scheiter, K. (2003). Goal configurations and processing strategies as moderators between instructional design and cognitive load: Evidence from hypertext-based instruction.Educational Psychologist, 38, 33–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (1996). Changing the agency for learning: Acquiring expert performance. In K.A. Ericsson (Ed.),The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 303–311). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect.Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design.Human Factors, 40, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2005). Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development 53(3), 83–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments.Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Tuovinen, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Darabi, A. (2005). A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learners' involvement in instructional conditions. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development, 53(3), 25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations.Learning and Instruction, 12, 529–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rikers, R.M.J.P., Schmidt, H.G., & Boshuizen, H.P.A. (2002). On the constraints of encapsulated knowledge: Clinical case representations by medical experts and subexperts.Cognition and Instruction, 20, 27–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rikers, R.M.J.P., van Gerven, P.W.M., & Schmidt, H.G. (2004). Cognitive load theory as a tool for expertise development.Instructional Science, 32, 173–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salden, R.J.C.M., Paas, F., Broers, N.J., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Mental effort and performance as determinants for the dynamic selection of learning tasks in air traffic control training.Instructional Science, 32, 153–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shute, V., & Towle, B. (2003). Adaptive e-learningEducational Psychologist, 38, 105–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture.Instructional Science, 32, 9–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design.Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding.Instructional Science, 32, 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, X., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing.Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B.J. (2002). Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 85–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tamara van Gog.

Additional information

In their preparation of this article, the first and last authors were supported by an Internationalization grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, The Hague, project number 411-01-010).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Gog, T., Ericsson, K.A., Rikers, R.M.J.P. et al. Instructional design for advanced learners: Establishing connections between the theoretical frameworks of cognitive load and deliberate practice. ETR&D 53, 73–81 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504799

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504799

Keywords

Navigation