Skip to main content
Log in

A computational approach to socially distributed cognition

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In most Interactive Learning Environments (ILEs), the human learner interacts with an expert in the domain to be taught. We explored a different approach: the system does not know more than the learner, but learns by interacting with him. A human-computer collaborative learning (HCCL) system includes a micro-world, in which two learners jointly try to solve problems and learn, the human learner and a computerized co-learner. This paper presents the foundations of this artificial co-learner.

The collaboration between learners is modelled as “socially distributed cognition’ (SDC). The SDC model connects three ideas: (i) a group is a cognitive system, (ii) reflection is a dialogue with oneself, (iii) social processes are internalised. The key has been to find a computational connection between those ideas. The domain chosen for illustration is the argumentation concerning how some changes to an electoral system affect the results of elections. This argumentation involves a sequence of arguments and their refutations. The basic principle is that learners ‘store’ the structure of this argumentation (dialogue pattern) and ‘replay’ it individually later on. The verbs ‘store’ and ‘replay’ do not refer to a simple ‘record and retrieve’ process. Storage is implemented as the incremental and parameterised evolution of a network of arguments, here called a ‘dialogue pattern’. The learning outcome is a structuration of knowledge (rules) into situation-specific models, used to guide reasoning.

We conducted experiments in two settings: with a human and an artificial learner or with two artificial learners. The common findings of these two experiments is that the SDC model generates learning effects provided that the discussion is intensive, i.e. that many arguments are brought into dialogue. The importance of this variable also appears in Hutchins’ (1991) modelling of the evolution of the confirmation bias in groups. It is argued than computational models are heuristic tools, allowing researchers to isolate variables for designing empirical studies with human subjects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, M. (1992).The collaborative construction of explanations. Paper presented to «Deuxièmes Journées Explication du PRC-GDR-IA du CNRS», Sophia-Antipolis, June 17–19 1992.

  • Blaye, A. (1988).Confrontation socio-cognitive et resolution de problèmes. Doctoral dissertation, Centre de Recherche en Psychologie Cognitive, Université de Provence, France.

  • Blaye, A., Light, P., Joiner, R., & Sheldon, S. (1991). Collaboration as a facilitator of planning and problem solving on a computer based task.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 471–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1982). A brief account of the conflict between the individual & the social in models of cognitive growth. In G. Butterworth & P. Light (Eds.),Social Cognition, (pp. 3–16). Brighton, Sussex: Harvester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, T.-W., & Baskin, A. B. (1988). «Studying with the prince», The computer as a learning companion.Proceedings of the International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems, (pp.194–200), June 1–3. Montreal, Canada.

  • Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991). Grounding in Communication. In L. Resnick, J. Levine, & S. Teasley,Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, (pp. 127–149) Hyattsville, MD: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P. R., & Feigenbaum, E. A. (Eds.) (1982).The Handbook of Artificial Intelligence, (Vol. III). Los Altos: Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillenbourg, P. (1991). Human-Computer Collaborative Learning.Doctoral dissertation. Department of Computing, University of Lancaster. Lancaster, U.K.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillenbourg, P. (1992). The computer as a contructorium: tools for observing one’s own learning. In R. Moyse & M. Elsom-Cook (Eds.),Knowledge Negotiation. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1984).The social development of the intellect. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durfee, E. H., Lesser, V. R., & Corkill, D. D. (1989). Cooperative Distributed Problem Solving. In A. Barr, P. R. Cohen & E. A. Feigenbaum (Eds.).The Handbook of Artificial Intelligence, (Vol. IV, pp. 83–127) Reading, Ma: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasser, L. (1991). Social conceptions of knowledge and action: DAI foundations and open systems semantics.Artificial Intelligence, 47, 107–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1991). The Social Organization of Distributed Cognition. In L. Resnick, J. Levine, & S. Teasley.Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, (pp. 383–307) Hyattsville, MD: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. (1983)Mental Models. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning.Cognitive Science, 12, 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934).Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michalski, R. S. (1986). Understanding the nature of learning: Issues & Research directions. In R. S. Michalski, J. G. Carbonell, & T. M. Mitchell (Eds.).Machine Learning. (Vol. II, pp. 3–26). Los Altos, California: Morgan Kaufman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1987).The society of mind. London: William Heinemann Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, N. (1986). Constructive Interaction and the Iterative, Process of Understanding.Cognitive Science, 10, 151–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyse, R. (1989). Knowledge Negotiation Implies Multiple Viewpoints.Proceedings of the 4th AI & Education Conference, (pp. 140–149), May 24–26. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D. (1989). Is a student model necessary? Apprenticeship as a model for ITS.Proceedings of the 4th AI & Education Conference, (pp. 177–184) May 24–26. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Malley, C. (1987). Understanding explanation.Paper presented at the third CeRCLe Workshop. Ullswater, U.K.

  • Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Perret, J.-F., & Bell, N. (1991). The Social Construction of Meaning and Cognitive Activity in Elementary School Children. In L. Resnick, J. Levine, & S. Teasley.Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, (pp. 41–62). Hyattsville, MD: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1928).The language and thought of the child. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B. (1991). Shared Cognition: Thinking as Social Practice. In L. Resnick, J. Levine, & S. Teasley.Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, (pp. 1–22). Hyattsville, MD: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J. (in press). Learning by Collaborating: Convergent Conceptual Change.Journal of the Learning Sciences.

  • Self, J. A. (1986). The application of machine learning to student modelling.Instructional Science, 14, 327–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L. A. (1987).Plans and Situated Actions. The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978).Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, Ma.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weizenbaum, J. (1966). ELIZA: a computer program for the study of natural language communication between man and machine.Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery, 29, 36–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Adult-Child Interaction as a Source of Self-Regulation in Children. In S. R. Yussen (Ed.),The growth of reflection in Children, (pp. 69–97). Madison, Wisconsin: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dillenbourg, P., Self, J.A. A computational approach to socially distributed cognition. Eur J Psychol Educ 7, 353–372 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172899

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172899

Key words

Navigation