Skip to main content
Log in

Ordering and structuring ideas in text: From conceptual organization to linguistic formulation

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The goal of this study was to analyze the development of the relationship between conceptual and linguistic processes as regards idea ordering and structuring (linearizing), when composing a text. Participants (from 7th graders to University students) were required to compose a text using a list of eleven scrambled ideas. Conceptual rules allow to a priori determine an optimal ordering of these ideas. Results showed a significant increase, with grade level, in the establishment of the postulated conceptual order, and in the linguistic elaboration of the text structure. A large autonomy was observed between conceptual and linguistic processes. The discussion considers the following points: The relevance of the notion of text optimal order; the unequal development with grade level of the varied linguistic skills; the dissymmetry in conceptual-linguistic relationships; the usefulness of the experimental paradigm; some consequences for education.

Résumé

Le but de cette étude consistait à analyser le développement de la relation entre facteurs conceptuels et facteurs linguistiques dans la mise en ordre et la structuration des idées d’un texte (processus de linéarisation). Les participants (élèves de 5ème à étudiants) devaient composer un texte à partir de 11 idées dont les relations avaient été préalablement choisies de façon à déterminer un ordre optimal du texte. Les résultats indiquent l’instauration progressive, avec le niveau scolaire, de l’ordre conceptuel optimal, ainsi qu’un accroissement significatif de l’élaboration linguistique. On observe par ailleurs une large autonomie entre processus conceptuels et processus linguistiques. La discussion examine les points suivants: la pertinence de la notion d’ordre textuel optimal; le développement inégal avec l’âge des différentes compétences linguistiques; la dissymétrie de la relation conceptuel-linguistique; l’utilité du paradigme expérimental; quelques conséquences dans le domaine de l’éducation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adam, J.M. (1992).Les textes: Types et prototypes — Récit, description, argumentation et dialogue. Paris: Nathan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akiguet, S., & Piolat, A. (1996). Insertion of connectives by 9-to 11-year-old children in an argumentative text.Argumentation, 10, 253–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2001).Through the Models of Writing. Dordrecht, Boston, London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andriessen, J. (1991).Minimal Strategies for Coherent Text Production. Utrecht: ISOR

    Google Scholar 

  • Andriessen, J., Coirier, P., Roos, L., Passerault, J.M., & Bert-Erboul, A. (1996). Thematic and structural planning in constrained argumentative text production. In R. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Berg, & M. Couzijn (Eds.),Theories, Models and Methodology in Writing Research (pp. 237–251). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apotheloz, D. (1990). The development of cohesion in writing: Preliminary research on anaphoric procedures and thematic planning in texts by children. In M. Spoelders (Ed.),A contribution of C&C to the International Litteracy Year (pp. 57–70). Lier: Van In and C & C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D.P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal material.Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 267–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987).The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V.W., & Swanson, H.L. (1994). Modification of the Hayes and Flower model to explain beginning and developing writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.),Advances in cognition and Educational Practice. Vol. 2: Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of development of skilled writing (pp. 57–82). Greenwich: CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J.K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax.Psychological Review, 89, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boscolo, P. (1995). The cognitive approach to writing and writing instruction: A contribution to a critical appraisal.Current Psychology of Cognition, 14(4), 343–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronckart, J.P., Bain, D., Schneuwly, B., Davaud, C., & Pasquier, A. (1985).Le fonctionnement du discours. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caccamise, D.J. (1987). Idea generation in writing. In A. Matsuhashi (Ed.),Writing in real time (pp. 224–253). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chanquoy, L. (1999). The use of connectives in different textual genres: A developmental analysis in a “thinking skills” perspective. In J.H.M. Hamers, J.E.H. van Luit, & B. Csapo (Eds.),Teaching and learning thinking skills. Contexts of learning (pp. 259–282). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chanquoy, L., & Fayol, M. (1995). Analyse de l’évolution de l’utilisation de la ponctuation et des connecteurs, dans deux types de texte. Etude longitudinale du C.P. au C.E.2.Enfance, 2, 227–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J.N., & Fayol, M. (1990). Temporal management of short text writing by children and adults.European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 513–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coirier, P. (1999). Les types de textes: Une approche de psychologie cognitive. In G. Skytte & F. Sabatini (Eds.),Linguistica testuale comparativa (pp. 11–36). Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coirier P., Broggio E., & De Bernardi, B. (1996). Systemic relations between compositional skills in 12-to 18-years-olds: Some empirical data. Oral communication to theE.A.R.L.I. SIG Writing Conference, Barcelona, Spain, October 23–25.

  • Coirier, P., & Favart, M. (2000). The development of conceptual-linguistic relationships in text composition: A replication. Oral communication to theE.A.R.L.I. SIG Writing Conference, Verona, September 7.

  • Dellerman, P., Coirier, P., & Marchand, E. (1996). Planning and expertise in argumentative composition. In R. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Berg, & M. Couzijn (Eds.),Theories, Models and Methodology in Writing Research (pp. 182–195). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Weck, G. (1991).La cohésion dans les textes d’enfants. Etude du développement des processus anaphoriques. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favart, M., & Chanquoy, L. (1998). How children ensure cohesion in written narrative and descriptive texts, with or without AND connective. Oral communication to theEARLI SIG Writing Conference, Poitiers, July 2–4.

  • Favart, M., & Coirier, P. (2000). Evolution of the linearization process in three types of texts. Oral communication to theE.A.R.L.I. SIG Writing Conference, Verona, September 7.

  • Favart, M., & Passerault, J.M. (1996). Functionality of cohesion devices in the management of local and global coherence: Two studies in children’s written production of narratives. In R. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Bergh, & M. Couzijn (Eds.),Theories, Models and Methodology in Writing Research (pp. 349–365). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favart, M., & Passerault, J.M. (1999). Aspects textuels du fonctionnement et du développement des connecteurs: Approche en production.L’Année Psychologique, 99, 149–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Favart, M., & Passerault, J.M. (2000). Aspects fonctionnels du point et de la virgule dans l’évolution de la planification du récit écrit.Enfance, 2, 187–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1985).Le récit et sa construction. Neuchâtel: Delachaux & Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1986). Les connecteurs dans les récits écrits. Etude chez l’enfant de 6 à 10 ans.Pratiques, 49, 101–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1989). Une approche psycholinguistique de la ponctuation. Etude en production et en compréhension.Langue Française, 81, 21–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1991). Text typologies: A cognitive approach. In G. Denhière & J.P. Rossi (Eds.),Texts and text processing (pp. 61–76). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Feilke, H. (1996). From syntactical to textual strategies of argumentation. Syntactical development in written argumentative texts by students aged 11 to 22.Argumentation, 10, 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, A., & Roussey A. (1994). Computer-assisted training effects on argumentative text writing skills in children. In G. Eigler & T. Jechle (Eds.),Writing: Current Trends in European Research (pp. 183–196). Freiburg: Hochschul Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J.R., & Flower, L.S. (1980a). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L.W. Gregg & E.R. Steinberg (Eds.),Cognitive process in writing: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J.R., & Flower, L.S. (1980b). Writing as problem solving.Visible Language, 4, 388–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isnard, N, & Piolat, A., (1994). The effects of different types of planning on the writing of argumentative text. In G. Eigler & T. Jechle (Eds.),Writing: Current trends in European Research (pp. 121–132). Freiburg: Hochsehul Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992).Beyond Modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive Science. Cambridge, London: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellog, R.T. (1987). Effects of topic knowledge on the allocation of processing time and cognitive effort to writing processes.Memory and Cognition, 15, 256–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R.T. (1990). Effectiveness of prewriting strategies as a function of task demands.American Journal of Psychology, 103(3), 327–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D.E. (1980). Initial mention as a signal to thematic content in technical passages.Memory and Cognition, 8, 345–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1977). On comprehending stories. In M. Just & P. Carpenter (Eds.),Cognitive Processes in comprehension (pp. 37–54). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozminsky, E. (1977). Altering comprehension: The effect of biasing titles on text comprehension.Memory and Cognition, 5, 482–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leitao, S. (2000). Evaluation and selection of ideas in argumentative writing. Oral communication to theThird International Workshop on Argumentative Text Processing, Verona, September 6.

  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1981).The speaker’s linearization problem. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. London, B295, 305–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R.F., & Lorch, E.P. (1985). Topic structure representation and text recall.Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLure, E., & Geva, E. (1983). The development of the cohesive use of adversative conjunctions in discourse.Discourse Processes, 6, 411–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1986). Domain knowledge in the development of writing ability.Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 2043–2058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1994). The magical number three plus or minus two: Working memory in writing. In J.S. Carlson & E.C. Butterfly (Eds.),Advances in Cognition and Educational Practice, Vol. 2: Children’s Writing: Toward a Process Theory of the Development of Skilled Writing (pp. 1–30). Greenwich, CT: J.A.I. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition.Educational Psychology Review, 8(3), 299–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuhashi, A. (1981). Pausing and planning: The tempo of written discourse production.Research in the teaching of English, 15, 113–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Passerault, J.M. (1986). Organisation du texte et stratégies d’intégration.Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 40, 263–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Passerault, J.M. (1991). La ponctuation: Recherches en psychologie du langage.Pratiques, 70, 85–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roussey, J.Y., & Gombert, A. (1996). Improving argumentative writing skills: Effect of two types of aids.Argumentation, 12–28.

  • Schneuwly, B. (1988).Le langage écrit chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scinto, L.F.M. (1984). Architectonics of texts produced by children and the development of higher cognitive function.Discourse Processes, 7, 371–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N., & Nezworski, T. (1978). The effects of organization and instructional set on story memory.Discourse Processes, 1, 178–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, J., Perkins, D.N., & Segal, J.W. (Eds.). (1991).Informal reasoning and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Coirier, P., Favart, M. & Chanquoy, L. Ordering and structuring ideas in text: From conceptual organization to linguistic formulation. Eur J Psychol Educ 17, 157–175 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173256

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173256

Key words

Navigation