Skip to main content
Log in

Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Consumers’ Support for the Fair Trade Movement

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite the reasonable explanatory power of existing models of consumers’ ethical decision making, a large part of the process remains unexplained. This article draws on previous research and proposes an integrated model that includes measures of the theory of planned behavior, personal norms, self-identity, neutralization, past experience, and attitudinal ambivalence. We postulate and test a variety of direct and moderating effects in the context of a large scale survey study in London, UK. Overall, the resulting model represents an empirically robust and holistic attempt to identify the most important determinants of consumers’ support for the fair-trade movement. Implications and avenues for further research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. The non-rationality of such motives has been disputed elsewhere, such as in game theory advancements (e.g. Camerer et al. 2011) and the work of the philosopher Bernard Williams (e.g. Smart and Williams 1973; Williams 1972).

  2. This correlation, which based on the work of Lindell and Whitney (2001), provides a stringent test.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (2002a). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Retrieved May 29, 2014, from www.people.umass.edu/aisen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf.

  • Ajzen, I. (2002b). Residual effects of past on later behavior: Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (2011). The theory of planned behavior: Reactions and reflections. Psychology & Health, 26(9), 1113–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Handbook of Attitudes (pp. 173–221). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Alexandrov, A. (2010). Characteristics of single-item measures in Likert scale format. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 8(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, W. T., & Cunningham, W. H. (1972). The socially conscious consumer. Journal of Marketing, 36(3), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andorfer, V. A., & Liebe, U. (2012). Research on fair trade consumption—A review. Journal of Business Ethics, 106(4), 415–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, C., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, H. J. (1982). Moderator variables: A clarification of conceptual, analytic and psychometric issues. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basil, D. Z., Ridgway, N. M., & Basil, M. D. (2006). Guilt appeals: The mediating effect of responsibility. Psychology and Marketing, 23(12), 1035–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bray, J., Johns, N., & Kilburn, D. (2011). An exploratory study into the factors impeding ethical consumption. Journal of Business Ethics, 98, 597–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, basic concepts, applications and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (Eds.). (2011). Advances in behavioral economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrigan, M., & Attalla, A. (2001). The myth of the ethical consumer—Do ethics matter in purchase behaviour? Journal of Consumer Marketing, 18(7), 560–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrington, M. J., Neville, B. A., & Whitwell, G. J. (2010). Why ethical consumers don’t walk their talk: Towards a framework for understanding the gap between the ethical purchase intentions and actual buying behavior of ethically minded consumers. Journal of Business Ethics, 97, 139–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrington, M. J., Neville, B. A., & Whitwell, G. J. (2014). Lost in translation: Exploring the ethical consumer intention–behavior gap. Journal of Business Research, 67(1), 2759–2767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castro, P., Garrido, M., Reis, E., & Menezes, J. (2009). Ambivalence and conservation behaviour: An exploratory study on the recycling of metal cans. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(1), 24–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, K. (1998). Mass communication and pro-environmental behaviour: Waste recycling in Hong Kong. Journal of Environmental Management, 52, 317–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, M. K. (1998). Predicting unethical behavior: A comparison of the theory of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(16), 1825–1834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charng, H.-W., Piliavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role identity and reasoned action in the prediction of repeated behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 303–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatzidakis, A., Hibbert, S., & Smith, A. P. (2007). Why people don’t take their concerns about fair trade to the supermarket: The role of neutralization. Journal of Business Ethics, 74, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatzidakis, A., Maclaran, P., & Bradshaw, A. (2012). Heterotopian space and the utopics of ethical and green consumption. Journal of Marketing Management, 28(3–4), 494–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cloake, F. (2014, May). Where are we to turn if we want to be ethical food shoppers these days? The Guardian, Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2014/may/24/ethical-food-shopping-fairtrade-report.

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M., Sparks, P., Povey, R., James, R., Shepherd, R., & Armitage, C. J. (2002). Moderator effects of attitudinal ambivalence on attitude–behaviour relationships. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(5), 705–718.

  • Costarelli, S., & Colloca, P. (2004). The effects of attitudinal ambivalence on pro-environmental behavioural intentions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 279–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cremin, C. (2012). The social logic of late capitalism: Guilt fetishism and the culture of crisis industry. Cultural Sociology, 6(1), 45–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darrow, A. L., & Kahl, D. R. (1982). A comparison of moderated regression techniques considering strength of effect. Journal of Management, 8, 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, J., Foxall, G. R., & Pallister, J. (2002). Beyond the intention–behaviour mythology an integrated model of recycling. Marketing Theory, 2(1), 29–113.

  • De Cannière, M. H., De Pelsmacker, P., & Geuens, M. (2009). Relationship quality and the theory of planned behavior models of behavioral intentions and purchase behavior. Journal of Business Research, 62, 82–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devinney, T. M., Auger, P., & Eckhardt, G. (2010). The myth of the ethical consumer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, B., Davies, I. A., & Tranchell, S. (2013). Where now for fair trade? Business History, 55(2), 161–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, D., & Norman, P. (2003). Predicting adolescent pedestrians’ road-crossing intentions: An application and extension of the theory of planned behaviour. Health Education Research, 18, 267–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fife-Schaw, C., Sheeran, P., & Norman, P. (2007). Simulating behaviour change interventions based on the theory of planned behaviour: Impacts on intention and action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 43–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, J. J., Eccles, M. P., Johnston, M., Walker, A., Grimshaw, J., Foy, R., et al. (2004a). Constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour: A manual for health services researchers. Newcastle: University of Newcastle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, J. J., Johnston, M., Eccles, M. P., Grimshaw, J., & Kaner, E. F. S. (2004b). Measurement issues in the theory of planned behaviour: A supplement to the manual for constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour. Newcastle: University of Newcastle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and mediator effects in counselling psychology research. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 51(1), 115–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fridell, G. (2009). The co-operative and the corporation: Competing visions of the future of fair trade. Journal of Business Ethics, 86(1), 81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukukawa, K. (2002). Developing a framework for ethically questionable behavior in consumption. Journal of Business Ethics, 14, 99–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godin, G., Conner, M., & Sheeran, P. (2005). Bridging the intention–behaviour “gap”: The role of moral norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 497–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2006). Self-identity and the theory of planned behaviour: Between- and within-participants analyses. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 731–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. (2001). The influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19(9), 711–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., & Biddle, S. J. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24(1), 3–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haight, C., & Henderson, D. R. (2010). Fair trade is counterproductive and unfair: Rejoinder. Economic Affairs, 30(1), 88–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (2009). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R., Newholm, T., & Shaw, D. (Eds.). (2005). The ethical consumer. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassan, L., Shaw, D., & Shiu, D. (2014). Who says there is an intention-behaviour gap? Assessing the empirical evidence of an intention-behaviour gap in ethical consumption. Journal of Business Ethics, forthcoming.

  • Henderson, D. R. (2008). Fair trade is counterproductive—and unfair. Economic Affairs, 28(3), 62–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., Sparkman, R. D, Jr, & Wilcox, J. B. (1982). The pretest in survey research: Issues and preliminary Findings. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(2), 269–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of Macromarketing, 6, 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (1992). The general theory of marketing ethics: A retrospective and revision. In C. Smith & J. Quelch (Eds.), Ethics in marketing (pp. 775–784). Homewood, IL: Irwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (2006). The general theory of marketing ethics: A revision and three Questions. Journal of Macromarketing, 26(2), 143–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, C., Smith, A., & Conner, M. (2003). Applying an extended version of the theory of planned behaviour to physical activity. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21(2), 119–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, F. G., Ranney, M., Hartig, T., & Bowler, P. A. (1999). Ecological behaviour, environmental attitude, and feelings of responsibility for the environment. European Psychologist, 4(2), 59–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, F. G., & Shimoda, T. A. (1999). Responsibility as a predictor of ecological behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 19, 243–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalafatis, S. P., Pollard, M., East, R., & Tsogas, M. H. (1999). Green marketing and Ajzen’s theory of planned behaviour: A cross-market examination. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16(5), 441–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keynote Report. (2012). Green and ethical consumer market assessment. Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://www.keynote.co.uk/market-intelligence/view/product/10537/green-and-ethical-consumer?medium=download.

  • Keynote Report. (2013). Green and ethical consumer market update. Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://www.keynote.co.uk/market-intelligence/view/product/10854/green-%26-ethical-consumer.

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraft, P., Rise, J., Sutton, S., & Røysamb, E. (2005). Perceived difficulty in the theory of planned behaviour: Perceived behavioural control or affective attitude? British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 479–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krier, J.-M. (2001). Fair trade in Europe 2001: Facts and figures on the fair trade sector in 18 European countries. Maastricht: EFTA Research Report.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 114–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovelock, C. H., Stiff, R., Cullwick, D., & Kaufman, I. M. (1976). An evaluation of the effectiveness of the drop-off questionnaire delivery. Journal of Marketing Research, 13(4), 358–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, W., & Davenport, E. (2005). Has the medium (roast) become the message? The ethics of marketing fair trade in the mainstream. International Marketing Research, 22(5), 494–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malhotra, N. K., Kim, S. S., & Patil, A. (2006). Common method variance in IS research: A comparison of alternative approaches and a reanalysis of past research. Management Science, 52(12), 1865–1883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manstead, A. S. R., & Parker, D. (1995). Evaluating and extending the theory of planned behaviour. European Review of Social Psychology, 6(1), 69–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interaction and moderation effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 376–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintel Report. (2007). Green and ethical consumers. Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://oxygen.mintel.com/display/221206/.

  • Mohr, J. J., Fisher, R. J., & Nevin, J. R. (1996). Collaborative communication in interfirm relationships: moderating effects of integration and control. Journal of Marketing, 60(3), 103–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, A., & Lee, N. (2006). Purchase decision making in fair trade and the ethical purchase “gap”: “Is there a fair-trade Twix?”. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 14, 369–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, P., & Conner, M. (2006). The theory of planned behaviour and binge drinking: Assessing the moderating role of past behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11(1), 55–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, P., Conner, M., & Bell, R. (2000). The theory of planned behaviour and exercise: Evidence for the moderating role of past behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5(3), 249–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Notani, A. S. (1998). Moderators of perceived behavioral control’s predictiveness in the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 247–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, J. (2003). Some problems with social cognition models: A pragmatic and conceptual analysis. Health Psychology, 22, 424–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozcaglar-Toulouse, N., Shiu, E., & Shaw, D. (2006). In search of fair trade: Ethical consumer decision making in France. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 30(5), 502–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended biases. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. A., & Wong, N. (2008). The safety of objects: Materialism, existential insecurity and brand connection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossiter, J. R. (2002). The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19(4), 305–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoonhoven, C. B. (1981). Problems with contingency theory: Testing assumptions hidden within the language of contingency “theory”. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26, 349–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221–279). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H., & Howard, J. A. (1980). Explanations of the moderating effect of responsibility denial on the personal norm-behavior relationship. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43(4), 441–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H., & Howard, J. A. (1981). A normative decision-making model of altruism. In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 189–211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D., & Clarke, I. (1999). Belief formation in ethical consumer groups: An exploratory study. Marketing Intelligence and Planning, 17(2), 109–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2002a). An assessment of ethical obligation and self-identity in ethical consumer decision-making: A structural equation modeling approach. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 26(4), 286–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2002b). The role of ethical obligation and self-identity in ethical consumer choice. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 26(2), 109–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2003). Ethics in consumer choice: A multivariate modelling approach. European Journal of Marketing, 37(10), 1485–1498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D., Shiu, E., & Clarke, I. (2000). The contribution of ethical obligation and self-identity to the theory of planned behaviour: An exploration of ethical consumers. Journal of Marketing Management, 16, 879–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart, J. J. C., & Williams, B. (1973). Utilitarianism for & against. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. (2009). Evaluating the criticisms of fair trade. Economic Affairs, 29(4), 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, P., & Guthrie, C. A. (1998). Self-identity and the theory of planned behavior: A useful addition or an unhelpful artifice? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1393–1410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, P., & Shepherd, R. (2002). The role of moral judgments within expectancy-value-based attitude-behavior models. Ethics and Behavior, 12(4), 299–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., & Frewer, L. J. (1995). Assessing and structuring attitudes toward the use of gene technology in food production: The role of perceived ethical obligation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16(3), 267–285.

  • Strong, C. (1996). Features contributing to the growth of ethical consumerism: A preliminary investigation. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 14(5), 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about answers. The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22(6), 664–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of planned behaviour: Self-identity, social identity and group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 225–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, D., Sheeran, P., Conner, M., & Finlay, K. A. (2002). Evidence that perceived behavioural control is a multidimensional construct: Perceived control and perceived difficulty. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(1), 101–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., Orwin, R., & Schurer, L. (1982). Defensive denial and high cost prosocial behaviour. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 3(4), 267–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? European Review of Social Psychology, 10(1), 101–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitell, S. J., & Ho, F. N. (1997). Ethical decision making in marketing: A synthesis and evaluation of scales measuring the various components of decision making in ethical situations. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 699–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, F. E. (1975). Developing the characteristics of the socially conscious consumer. Journal of Consumer Research, 2, 185–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. (1972). Knowledge and reasons. Problems in the theory of knowledge/problèmes de la théorie de la connaissance (pp. 1–11). Hague, Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Yeow, P., Dean, A., & Tucker, D. (2013). Bags for life: The embedding of ethical consumerism. Journal of Business Ethics,. doi:10.1007/s10551-013-1900-2.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Chatzidakis.

Appendix: Excerpts from the Survey Instrument and Explanation of the Items

Appendix: Excerpts from the Survey Instrument and Explanation of the Items

Intention Following Francis et al.’s (2004) suggested format, we assessed general intention to support fair trade using three items:

  • “I expect to support the fair trade movement in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I want to support the fair trade movement in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I intend to support the fair trade movement in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

We used three additional items to measure intentions for specific behaviors:

  • “I would support the fair trade movement in the near future, by buying fair trade products”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I would support the fair trade movement in the near future, by signing a petition for fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I would support the fair trade movement in the near future, by donating to the fair trade organization”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

Attitudes We assessed attitudes by employing a semantic differential scale, as Ajzen (2002a) suggests. Respondents were presented with the statement, “Supporting the fair trade movement is …”, followed by seven pairs of adjectives: harmful/beneficial, good/bad, pleasant/unpleasant, worthless/valuable, enjoyable/unenjoyable, rewarding/not rewarding, and the right thing to do/the wrong thing to do. An additional question, “In general, my attitude towards fair trade is …” was followed by two pairs of adjectives intended to capture overall evaluation (Sparks and Shepherd 2002): unfavorable/favorable (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) and negative/positive (e.g. Sparks and Shepherd 2002).

Subjective norms We measured subjective norms with five sentences following the recommendations of Ajzen (2002a):

  • “Most people who are important to me support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “Most people who are important to me think that I should support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “The people in my life whose opinions I value would not approve of my supporting for fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “The people in my life whose opinions I value support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “It is expected of me that I support fair trade in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

Perceived behavioral control We measured perceived behavioral control with four statements (Ajzen 2002a):

  • “For me to support the fair trade movement in the near future would be difficult”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “If I wanted to I could support the fair trade movement in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “It is mostly up to me whether or not I support fair trade in the near future”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “How much control do you believe you have over supporting fair trade in the near future”? (“no control/complete control”).

Personal norms We measured personal norms with three questions:

  • “I feel that I have an ethical/moral obligation to support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I personally feel I should support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “Supporting the fair trade movement would be the right thing for me to do”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

The first question retained the suggested format of Sparks et al. (1995), while the second and third were of similar format to measures employed by Sparks and Guthrie (1998) and Davies et al. (2002).

Self-identity We constructed three questions to assess self-identification with fair-trade issues:

  • “To support fair trade is an important part of who I am”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I think of myself as someone who is concerned about ethical issues in consumption”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I am not the type of person oriented to support fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

The first two questions retained the suggested format of Terry et al. (1999), and the third was based on the wording used by Sparks and Shepherd (2002).

Neutralisation We measured neutralization with three questions meant to capture “justifiability” of not supporting fair trade, following Chatzidakis et al. (2007):

  • “For me, not supporting fair trade is justifiable”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I have many arguments against supporting fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”)

  • “I’ve got reasons for not supporting fair trade”. (“strongly disagree/strongly agree”).

Past behavior We assessed past behavior with a variety of differently worded questions, as Ajzen (2002a) recommends:

  • In the course of the past 3 months, how many times have you decided to support the Fair Trade movement? (please tick one statement)

    • Every time that I had the opportunity. ____

    • Almost every time that I had the opportunity. ____

    • Most of the time that I had the opportunity. ____

    • About half of the times that I had the opportunity. ____

    • Sometimes, but less than half of the times I had the opportunity. ____

    • Few times that I had the opportunity. ____

    • Not at all when I had the opportunity. ____

    • I have not had the opportunity. ____

  • How often do you support the Fair Trade Movement? (“never/always”)

  • How often do you purchase Fair Trade products? (“never/always”)

  • Have you ever bought Fair Trade products? (please tick one)

Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have not had the opportunity __

  • Have you ever signed a petition for Fair Trade? (please tick one)

Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have not had the opportunity__

  • Have you ever donated to the Fair Trade Organization? (please tick one)

Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have not had the opportunity__

  • Have you ever supported Fair Trade through other ways? (please tick one)

Yes __ No __ If yes, please specify:__

Ambivalence We measured ambivalence with five questions

  • Regarding supporting the Fair Trade movement I feel that my attitude is…. (“not at all contradictory/very contradictory”)

  • Considering only the unfavorable qualities of Fair Trade and ignoring the favorable characteristics, how unfavorable is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade movement? (“not at all unfavorable/extremely unfavorable”).

  • Considering only the negative qualities of Fair Trade and ignoring the positive characteristics, how negative is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade movement? (“not at all negative/extremely negative”)

  • Considering only the favorable qualities of Fair Trade and ignoring the unfavorable characteristics, how favorable is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade movement? (“not at all favorable/extremely favorable”)

  • Considering only the positive qualities of Fair Trade and ignoring the negative characteristics, how positive is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade movement? (“not at all positive/extremely positive”).

The first question retained the format used in Castro et al. (2009); we adapted the rest of the questions from Conner et al. (2002).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chatzidakis, A., Kastanakis, M. & Stathopoulou, A. Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Consumers’ Support for the Fair Trade Movement. J Bus Ethics 133, 95–109 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2347-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2347-9

Keywords

Navigation