Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of Negative Message Framing on Green Consumption: An Investigation of the Role of Shame

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite society’s increasing sensitivity toward green production, companies often struggle to find effective communication strategies that induce consumers to buy green products or engage in other environmentally friendly behaviors. To add clarity to this situation, we investigated the effectiveness of negative versus positive message framing in promoting green products, whereby companies highlight the detrimental versus beneficial environmental consequences of choosing less versus more green options, respectively. Across four experiments, we show that negatively framed messages are more effective than positively framed ones in prompting consumers to engage in pro-environmental behaviors. More importantly, we find that anticipated shame is the emotion responsible for this effect. Furthermore, both environmental concern and the type of product promoted serve as moderators; thus, the mediating role of anticipated shame is attenuated when environmental concern is low and the product is a luxury one. Finally, we discuss the theoretical and managerial implications of our work, along with its limitations and some directions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achabou, M. A., & Dekhili, S. (2013). Luxury and sustainable development: Is there a match? Journal of Business Research, 66(10), 1896–1903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Agrawal, N., & Duhachek, A. (2010). Emotional compatibility and the effectiveness of antidrinking messages: A defensive processing perspective on shame and guilt. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 263–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alden, D. L., & Crowley, A. E. (1995). Sex guilt and receptivity to condom advertising. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(16), 1446–1463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amatulli, C., De Angelis, M., Costabile, M., & Guido, G. (2017). Sustainable luxury brands: Evidence from research and implications for managers. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arthur, D., & Quester, P. (2004). Who’s afraid of that ad? Applying segmentation to the protection motivation model. Psychology & Marketing, 21(9), 671–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., & Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 184–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. P., & Pieters, R. (1998). Goal-directed emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12(1), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, S. M., Salovey, P., Greener, S., Rothman, A. J., Moyer, A., Beauvais, J., et al. (1995). The effects of message framing on mammography utilization. Health Psychology, 14(2), 178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basil, D. Z., Ridgway, N. M., & Basil, M. D. (2006). Guilt appeals: The mediating effect of responsibility. Psychology & Marketing, 23(12), 1035–1054.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basil, D. Z., Ridgway, N. M., & Basil, M. D. (2008). Guilt and giving: A process model of empathy and efficacy. Psychology & Marketing, 25(1), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bécheur, I., Dib, H., Merunka, D., & Valette-Florence, P. (2007). Emotions of fear, guilt or shame in anti-alcohol messages: Measuring direct effects on persuasion and the moderating role of sensation seeking. In S. Borghini, M. A. McGrath, & C. Otnes (Eds.), European advances in consumer research (Vol. 8, pp. 99–106). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, R. (1996). Effects of horrific fear appeals on public attitudes towards AIDS. International Journal of Advertising, 15(3), 183–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, R. (1998). Shame, guilt & responses to non-profit & public sector ads. International Journal of Advertising, 17(4), 483–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, I. E., & Corbin, R. M. (1992). Perceived consumer effectiveness and faith in others as moderators of environmentally responsible behaviors. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 11(2), 79–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, L. G., & Keller, P. A. (1995). When to accentuate the negative: The effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions to perform a health-related behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 32(2), 192–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bozinoff, L., & Ghingold, M. (1983). Evaluating guilt arousing marketing communications. Journal of Business Research, 11(2), 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, L., & Binney, W. (2010). Fear, guilt, and shame appeals in social marketing. Journal of Business Research, 63(2), 140–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cervellon, M. C. (2013). Conspicuous conservation. International Journal of Market Research, 55(5), 695–717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandy, R. K., Tellis, G. J., MacInnis, D. J., & Thaivanich, P. (2001). What to say when: Advertising appeals in evolving markets. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(4), 399–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, H., Zhang, L., & Xie, G. X. (2015). Message framing in green advertising: The effect of construal level and consumer environmental concern. International Journal of Advertising, 34(1), 158–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhuri, A. (1998). Product class effects on perceived risk: The role of emotion. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 15(2), 157–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cotte, J., Coulter, R. A., & Moore, M. (2005). Enhancing or disrupting guilt: The role of ad credibility and perceived manipulative intent. Journal of Business Research, 58(3), 361–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cristini, H., Kauppinen-Räisänen, H., Barthod-Prothade, M., & Woodside, A. (2017). Toward a general theory of luxury: Advancing from workbench definitions and theoretical transformations. Journal of Business Research, 70(1), 101–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dale, A. (2008). Enterprise: Green products gain from new price equation; they find new buyers as high energy costs hurt regular brands. Wall Street Journal – Eastern Edition, 251, B7 (June 24, 2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, I. A., Lee, Z., & Ahonkhai, I. (2012). Do consumers care about ethical-luxury? Journal of Business Ethics, 106(1), 37–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. J. (1995). The effects of message framing on response to environmental communications. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 72(2), 285–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Angelis, M., Adıgüzel, F., & Amatulli, C. (2017). The role of design similarity in consumers’ evaluation of new green products: An investigation of luxury fashion brands. Journal of Cleaner Production, 141, 1515–1527.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Barnier, V., Falcy, S., & Valette-Florence, P. (2012). Do consumers perceive three levels of luxury? A comparison of accessible, intermediate and inaccessible luxury brands. Journal of Brand Management, 19(7), 623–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2008). Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 933–943.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2010). Restore and protect motivations following shame. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2011). A functionalist account of shame-induced behaviour. Cognition and Emotion, 25(5), 939–946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dearing, R. L., Stuewig, J., & Tangney, J. P. (2005). On the importance of distinguishing shame from guilt: Relations to problematic alcohol and drug use. Addictive Behaviors, 30(7), 1392–1404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delmas, M. A., & Cuerel Burbano, V. (2011). The drivers of greenwashing. California Management Review, 54(1), 64–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duhachek, A., Agrawal, N., & Han, D. (2012). Guilt versus shame: Coping, fluency, and framing in the effectiveness of responsible drinking messages. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(6), 928–941.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, E., & Gallagher, L. (2007). Message framing with respect to decisions about vaccination: The roles of frame valence, frame method and perceived risk. British Journal of Psychology, 98(4), 667–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M., & Haley, K. J. (2003). The strategy of affect: Emotions in human cooperation. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 7–36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R. J. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 992–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. N., Kennelly, J. J., & Krause, T. S. (1995). Shifting paradigms for sustainable development: Implications for management theory and research. Academy of Management Review, 20(4), 874–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 392–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Han, D., Duhachek, A., & Agrawal, N. (2014). Emotions shape decisions through construal level: The case of guilt and shame. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(4), 1047–1064.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haws, K., Winterich, K. P., & Naylor, R. W. (2014). Seeing the world through GREEN-tinted glasses: Green consumption values and responses to environmentally friendly products. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(3), 336–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67(3), 451–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homer, P. M., & Yoon, S. (1992). Message framing and the interrelationships among ad-based feelings, affect, and cognition. Journal of Advertising, 21(1), 19–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jain, S. P., Lindsey, C., Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2007). For better or for worse? Valenced comparative frames and regulatory focus. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 57–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, W. S., & Villegas, J. (2011). The effects of message framing, involvement, and nicotine dependence on anti-smoking public service announcements. Health Marketing Quarterly, 28(3), 219–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalamas, M., Cleveland, M., & Laroche, M. (2014). Pro-environmental behaviors for thee but not for me: Green giants, green Gods, and external environmental locus of control. Journal of Business Research, 67(2), 12–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapferer, J. N., & Michaut-Denizeau, A. (2014). Is luxury compatible with sustainability? Luxury consumers’ viewpoint. Journal of Brand Management, 21(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketelaar, T., & Tung Au, W. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17(3), 429–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kronrod, A., Grinstein, A., & Wathieu, L. (2012). Go green! Should environmental messages be so assertive? Journal of Marketing, 76(1), 95–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46(4), 352–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y. C., & Chang, C. C. A. (2012). Double standard: The role of environmental consciousness in green product usage. Journal of Marketing, 76(5), 125–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay-Hartz, J., De Rivera, J., & Mascolo, M. F. (1995). Differentiating guilt and shame and their effects on motivation. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 274–300). New York: Guildford.

    Google Scholar 

  • List, S. K. (1993). The green seal of eco-approval. American Demographics, 15(1), 9–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, K. R. (1994). Motivating recycling behavior: A quasiexperimental investigation of message and source strategies. Psychology & Marketing, 11(4), 341–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lozano, R. (2008). Envisioning sustainability three-dimensionally. Journal of Cleaner Production, 16(17), 1838–1846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maheswaran, D., & Meyers-Levy, J. (1990). The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of Marketing Research, 27(3), 361–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchand, J., & Filiatrault, P. (2002). AIDS prevention advertising: Different message strategies for different communication objectives. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 7(3), 271–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyerowitz, B. E., & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 500–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyers-Levy, J., & Maheswaran, D. (2004). Exploring message framing outcomes when systematic, heuristic, or both types of processing occur. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(1), 159–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaelidou, N., & Hassan, L. M. (2008). The role of health consciousness, food safety concern and ethical identity on attitudes and intentions towards organic food. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 32(2), 163–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moons, I., & De Pelsmacker, P. (2012). Emotions as determinants of electric car usage intention. Journal of Marketing Management, 28(3/4), 195–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, K., McGregor, I., & Prentice, M. (2011). Threat and defense as goal regulation: From implicit goal conflict to anxious uncertainty, reactive approach motivation, and ideological extremism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1291–1301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. M., & Meijers, M. H. (2011). Social benefits of luxury brands as costly signals of wealth and status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(5), 343–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). “If only I weren’t” versus “If only I hadn’t”: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 585–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obermiller, C. (1995). The baby is sick/the baby is well: A test of environmental communication appeals. Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 55–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olsen, M. C., Slotegraaf, R. J., & Chandukala, S. R. (2014). Green claims and message frames: How green new products change brand attitude. Journal of Marketing, 78(5), 119–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Organic Cotton Report (2013). Textile exchange 2013 organic cotton reportpress release. http://textileexchange.org/sites/default/files/OCR%20Press%20Release.pdf. Accessed January 20, 2016.

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1990). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, V. M., Chun, H. E., & MacInnis, D. J. (2009). Affective forecasting and self-control: When anticipating pride wins over anticipating shame in a self-regulation context. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 537–545.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perloff, R. M. (2010). The dynamics of persuasion: Communication and attitudes in the twenty-first century. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M. T., Cohen, J. B., Pracejus, J. W., & Hughes, G. D. (2001). Affect monitoring and the primacy of feelings in judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(2), 167–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pizzolatto, A. B., & Zeringue, C. A., II. (1993). Facing society’s demands for environmental protection: Management in practice. Journal of Business Ethics, 12(6), 441–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberto, A. J., Meyer, G., Johnson, A. J., & Atkin, C. K. (2000). Using the parallel process model to prevent firearm injury and death: Field experiment results of a video-based intervention. Journal of Communication, 50(4), 157–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romani, S., Grappi, S., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2016). Corporate socially responsible initiatives and their effects on consumption of green products. Journal of Business Ethics, 135(2), 253–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 5(3), 161–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossiter, J. R., & Thornton, J. (2004). Fear-pattern analysis supports the fear-drive model for antispeeding road-safety TV ads. Psychology & Marketing, 21(11), 945–960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Keough, K., & Martin, C. D. (1993). The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(5), 408–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (1998). Emotion, character, and responsibility. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1993). Studying the emotion-antecedent appraisal process: An expert system approach. Cognition and Emotion, 7(3/4), 325–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuhwerk, M. E., & Lefkoff-Hagius, R. (1995). Green or non-green? Does type of appeal matter when advertising a green product? Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 45–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Edell Britton, J. A., & Payne, J. W. (1997). Factors affecting the impact of negatively and positively framed ad messages. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(3), 285–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., Edell Britton, J. A., & Payne, J. W. (2004). Does elaboration increase or decrease the effectiveness of negatively versus positively framed messages? Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 199–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105(1), 131–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P. (1995). Recent advances in the empirical study of shame and guilt. The American Behavioral Scientist, 38(8), 1132–1145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P. (1996). Conceptual and methodological issues in the assessment of shame and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(9), 741–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and Guilt (Emotions and Social Behavior). New York: Guildford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Are shame and guilt related to distinct self-discrepancies? A test of Higgins’s (1987) hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 256–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. R. (2015). A call for more research on “green” or environmental advertising. International Journal of Advertising, 34(4), 573–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theotokis, A., & Manganari, E. (2015). The impact of choice architecture on sustainable consumer behavior: The role of guilt. Journal of Business Ethics, 131(2), 423–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torelli, C. J., Monga, A. B., & Kaikati, A. M. (2012). Doing poorly by doing good: Corporate social responsibility and brand concepts. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(5), 948–963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toyota (2015). Toyota environmental challenge 2050. http://www.toyota-global.com/sustainability/environment/challenge2050/. Accessed February 09, 2016.

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: Support for a theoretical model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 1339–1351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Doorn, J., & Verhoef, P. C. (2011). Willingness to pay for organic products: Differences between virtue and vice foods. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 28(3), 167–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigneron, F., & Johnson, L. W. (2004). Measuring perceptions of brand luxury. Journal of Brand Management, 11(6), 484–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., & Wallendorf, M. (2006). Materialism, status signaling, and product satisfaction. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 34(4), 494–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, K., MacDonnell, R., & Dahl, D. W. (2011). It’s the mind-set that matters: The role of construal level and message framing in influencing consumer efficacy and conservation behaviors. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3), 472–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widloecher, P. (2010). Luxe et développement durable: Je t’aime, moi non plus? Luxefrancais. http://www.femininbio.com/agir-green/actualites-et-nouveautes/luxe-et-developpement-durable-je-taime-moi-non-plus-61430. Accessed 30 May 2017.

  • Wolin, L. D. (2003). Gender issues in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 43(1), 111–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, D., Lu, Y., Zhu, W., & Su, C. (2015). Going green: How different advertising appeals impact green consumption behavior. Journal of Business Research, 68(12), 2663–2675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zemack-Rugar, Y., Rabino, R., Cavanaugh, L. A., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2016). When donating is liberating: The role of product and consumer characteristics in the appeal of cause-related products. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(2), 213–230.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cesare Amatulli.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Human and Animal Rights

All procedures performed in the studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee, as well as with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Additional information

The first three authors are listed in alphabetical order and contributed equally to the article.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Study 1

Framing Valence Manipulation

Illustrative Screenshots of the Video

Illustrative screenshot of the video showed to female respondents

Illustrative screenshot of the video showed to male respondents

Transcripts from the video

Presentation in common among the three conditions

According to you, do the traditional batteries that we use for remotes, alarm clocks, cordless phones, etc., pollute the environment when we waste them?

A battery contains about one gram of mercury, more than enough to pollute 1000 L of water!

In stores you can generally find two versions of batteries:

These ones, which cannot be recharged, thus becoming an environmental risk factor; these batteries are not sustainable from an environmental point of view, so they are polluting.

And these ones, which, in contrast, are rechargeable, produced with materials that do not pollute even if wasted in the environment; these batteries guarantee the same performance as the others, but cost 20% more; certainly, however, they do not pollute the environment.

 

Concluding instruction used in the negative framing condition

So, if you choose to buy the first version of batteries, that is, the ones that are polluting, instead of the second version, you will deeply contribute to the destruction of the environment and to the bad living conditions of future generations.

 

Concluding instruction used in the positive framing condition

So if you choose to buy the second version of batteries, that is, the ones that are not polluting, instead of the first version, you will help the environment and concretely contribute to the well-being of future generations.

 

Concluding instruction used in the neutral framing condition

(No information was given in this condition about the consequences for the environment.)

 

Appendix 2: Study 2

Framing Valence Manipulation

Part of the narrative scenario in common among the three conditions

Suppose you need a T-shirt and you are planning to purchase one.

You enter a store and see one model of T-shirt that you particularly like and you are seriously considering to buy. Nevertheless, while you keep walking in the store, you notice another T-shirt, manufactured by the same company and similar to the previous one except for the fact that it carries a certification to be made 100% with organic cotton. Therefore, the second T-shirt is objectively more environmentally friendly than the first one. However, such a second T-shirt costs 20% more than the former one.

 

Concluding instruction used in the negative framing condition

If you decide to buy the first T-shirt (the less environmentally friendly one) instead of the second you saw, you actively contribute to the collapse of the environment.

 

Concluding instruction used in the positive framing condition

If you decide to buy the second T-shirt (the more environmentally friendly one) instead of the first you saw, you certainly contribute to the improvement of the environment.

 

Concluding instruction used in the neutral framing condition

(No information was given in this condition about the consequences for the environment.)

 

Appendix 3: Study 3

Situational Environmental Concern Manipulation

Initial priming instruction used in the environmental concern salient condition

Congratulations, your mere participation to this research allows you to give a real contribution to advancing knowledge about the improvement in the environment.

 

Initial priming instruction used in the environmental concern non-salient condition

Participating to this questionnaire you have already made an important step in improving your cognitive and memory skills.

 

Framing Valence Manipulation

The same as in Study 2 (without the neutral condition).

Appendix 4: Study 4

Type of Product × Framing Valence Manipulation

Narrative scenario used in the luxury product and negative framing condition

Suppose you are planning to purchase a special luxury jacket that you plan to wear for special occasions, such as ceremonies or important social events where it is important for you to look cool in the eyes of others.

You enter a store and see one model of luxury jacket that you particularly like and you are seriously considering to buy. Nevertheless, while you keep walking in the store, you notice another luxury jacket, manufactured by the same prestigious Italian luxury brand and similar to the previous one except for the fact that it carries a certification to be made 100% with organic fabrics. Therefore, the second luxury jacket is objectively more environmentally friendly than the first one. However, such a second luxury jacket costs 20% more than the former one.

If you decide to buy the first luxury jacket (the less environmentally friendly one) instead of the second you saw, you certainly contribute to the collapse of the environment.

 

Narrative scenario used in the luxury product and positive framing condition

Suppose you are planning to purchase a special luxury jacket that you plan to wear for special occasions, such as ceremonies or important social events where it is important for you to look cool in the eyes of others.

You enter a store and see one model of luxury jacket that you particularly like and you are seriously considering to buy. Nevertheless, while you keep walking in the store, you notice another luxury jacket, manufactured by the same prestigious Italian luxury brand and similar to the previous one except for the fact that it carries a certification to be made 100% with organic fabrics. Therefore, the second luxury jacket is objectively more environmentally friendly than the first one. However, such a second luxury jacket costs 20% more than the former one.

If you decide to buy the second luxury jacket (the more environmentally friendly one) instead of the first you saw, you certainly contribute to the improvement of the environment.

 

Narrative scenario used in the non-luxury product and negative framing condition

Suppose you are planning to purchase an ordinary jacket that you plan to wear only for daily life, non-special activities, such as going to the grocery store or just staying at home with your family or close friends.

You enter a store and see one model of ordinary jacket that you particularly like and you are seriously considering to buy. Nevertheless, while you keep walking in the store, you notice another jacket, manufactured by the same company and similar to the previous one except for the fact that it carries a certification to be made 100% with organic fabrics. Therefore, the second jacket is objectively more environmentally friendly than the first one. However, such a second jacket costs 20% more than the former one.

If you decide to buy the first jacket (the less environmentally friendly one) instead of the second you saw, you certainly contribute to the collapse of the environment.

 

Narrative scenario used in the non-luxury product and positive framing condition

Suppose you are planning to purchase an ordinary jacket that you plan to wear only for daily life, non-special activities, such as going to the grocery store or just staying at home with your family or close friends.

You enter a store and see one model of ordinary jacket that you particularly like and you are seriously considering to buy. Nevertheless, while you keep walking in the store, you notice another jacket, manufactured by the same company and similar to the previous one except for the fact that it carries a certification to be made 100% with organic fabrics. Therefore, the second jacket is objectively more environmentally friendly than the first one. However, such a second jacket costs 20% more than the former one.

If you decide to buy the second jacket (the more environmentally friendly one) instead of the first you saw, you certainly contribute to the improvement of the environment.

 

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Amatulli, C., De Angelis, M., Peluso, A.M. et al. The Effect of Negative Message Framing on Green Consumption: An Investigation of the Role of Shame. J Bus Ethics 157, 1111–1132 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-017-3644-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-017-3644-x

Keywords

Navigation