Skip to main content
Log in

Specificity of Phonological Representations for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • OriginalPaper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated whether children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are sensitive to mispronunciations of familiar words and compared their sensitivity to children with typical-development. Sixty-four toddlers with ASD and 31 younger, typical controls participated in a looking-while-listening task that measured their accuracy in fixating the correct object when it was labelled with a correct pronunciation versus mispronunciation. A cognitive style that prioritizes processing local, rather than global features, as claimed by the weak central coherence theory, predicts that children with ASD should be more sensitive to mispronunciations than typical controls. The results, however, reveal no differences in the effect of mispronunciations on lexical processing between groups, even when matched for receptive language or non-verbal cognitive skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Consensus coding to achieve 100% comparable trials is important because low numbers of comparable trials may bias the measures of inter-coder agreement, particularly if trials that are not comparable are more difficult to code.

  2. The combined numbers of children in the matched and unmatched subsamples are less than the total number of children in our full sample. This is because there were extra children within each group who were matchable (i.e., their scores were within the range of the other group), but were not matched with another child from the other Group.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Philadelphia, PA: American Psychiatric Pub.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (1993). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., & Happé, F. (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 358, 387–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2010). “Hunting with a knife and … fork”: Examining central coherence in autism, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development with a linguistic task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 377–393.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108(3), 896–904.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S., & Bebko, J. (2012). Generalization, overselectivity, and discrimination in the autism phenotype: A review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 733–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceponiene, R., Lepistö, T., Shestakova, A., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Näätänen, R., et al. (2003). Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: They can perceive but do not attend. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 5567–5572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children’s lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 17, 205–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., Yang, D., Gray, T. L., Gross, M. M., Abbacchi, A. M., Smith, S. C., et al. (2007). Clarifying the associations between language and social Development in autism: A study of non-native phoneme recognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1256–1263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early Social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 331–343.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DePape, A.-M. R., Hall, G. B. C., Tillman, B., & Trainor, L. J. (2012). Auditory processing in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 7, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhardt, M., & Nadig, A. (2016). Reduced sensitivity to context in language comprehension: A characteristic of autism spectrum disorders or of poor structural language ability? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 72(2018), 284–296.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis Weismer, S., Haebig, E., Edwards, J., Saffran, J., & Venker, C. E. (2016). Lexical processing in toddlers with ASD: Does weak central coherence play a role? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 3755–3769.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P. S., Marchman, V. A., Reznick, J. S., & Thal, D. J. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech Processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language. Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-line Methods in Children’s Language Processing, 44, 97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomot, M., & Wicker, B. (2012). A challenging, unpredictable world for people with Autism Spectrum Disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 240–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, R., Zheng, S., Kinard, J., Mosner, M., Wiesen, C., Kennedy, D., et al. (2019). Social and nonsocial visual prediction errors in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 5, 2. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hala, S., Pexman, P., & Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 329–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in Context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Booth, R. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Powell, A., Gaskell, M. G., & Norbury, C. (2014). Learning and consolidation of new spoken words in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17, 858–871.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoy, J. A., Hatton, C., & Hare, D. (2004). Weak central coherence: A cross-domain phenomenon specific to autism? Autism, 8(3), 267–281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., Burack, J., Shore, D., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. (2006). Global-local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 117–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Ceponiene, R., Kielinen, M., Suominen, K., Jäntti, V., Linna, S.-L., et al. (2003). Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger Syndrome, as indexed by event-related potentials. Neuroscience Letters, 338, 197–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W. (1993). Some reflections on developmental changes in speech perception and production. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., Camfferman, G., & van Engeland, H. (1995). Auditory event-related brain potentials in autististic children and three different control groups. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 38, 150–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Key, A. P., Yoder, P. J., & Stone, W. L. (2016). Consonant differentiation mediates the discrepancy between nonverbal and verbal abilities in children with ASD. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 60, 478–490.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science, 8, F1–F12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lepistö, T., Kujala, T., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Huotilainen, M., & Näätänen, R. (2005). The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Research, 1066, 147–157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • López, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(2), 285–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part 1): Modules 1–4. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chanlder, S., Meldrum, D., et al. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1184–1192.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahr, T., McMillan, B., Saffran, J., Ellis Weismer, S., & Edwards, J. (2015). Anticipatory coarticulation facilitates word recognition in toddlers. Cognition, 142, 345–350.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2006). Fast-mapping in young children with autism spectrum disorders. First Language, 26, 421–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental re-structuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirman, D. (2016). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Burack, J. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J., Iarocci, G., Bellevile, S., & Enns, J. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(2), 142–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsycologia, 48, 4012–4019.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oram Cardy, J. E., Flagg, E. J., Roberts, W., & Roberts, T. P. (2005). Delayed mismatch field for speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 5, 521–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 719.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of the first birthday home video-tapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46, 530–544.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2011). Psychological models of autism: An overview. In I. Roth & P. Rezaie (Eds.), Researching the autism spectrum: Contemporary perspectives (pp. 219–265). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickles, A., Anderson, D., & Lord, C. (2014). Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: A 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 1354–1362.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K. (2000). Aspects of autism that theory of mind cannot easily explain. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 777–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plante, E., Swisher, L., Kiernan, B., & Restrepo, M. A. (1993). Language matches: Illuminating or confounding? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36, 772–776.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2016). Elephants in pyjamas: Testing the weak central coherence account of autism spectrum disorders using a syntactic disambiguation task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 155–163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire: Manual. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., et al. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. PNAS, 111(42), 15220–15225.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. E., & Ota, M. (2008). Lexical effects on speech perception in individuals with “autistic” traits. Cognition, 109, 157–162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D. (2005). 11-month-olds’ knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental science, 8, 432–443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13, 480–484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6, 468–478.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de Wit, L., et al. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Preditive coding in autism. Psychological Review, 121(4), 649–675.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venker, C. E., Edwards, J., Saffran, J., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2019). Thinking ahead: Incremental language processing is associated with receptive language abilities in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 1011–1023.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venker, C. E., Haebig, E., Edwards, J., Saffran, J. R., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2016). Brief report: Early lexical comprehension in young children with ASD: Comparing eye-gaze methodology and parent report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2260–2266.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home video-tapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 157–162.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • You, R. S., Serniclaes, W., Rider, D., & Chabane, N. (2017). On the nature of the speech perception deficits in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 61, 158–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool language scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the families and children who made this research possible. We thank Jessica Umhoefer and Heidi Sindberg for their clinical expertise. We also thank Tristan Mahr, Elizabeth Premo, Courtney Venker, and all other members of the Little Listeners Project team for their input and assistance.

Funding

This study was funded by an NIDCD Grant (RO1 DC012513) and an NICHD Core Grant to the Waisman Center (U54 HD090256).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RP drafted the manuscript, performed the statistical analysis, and led in the analysis and interpretation of the data; JE conceived of the study, led in its design and coordination, participated in the interpretation of the data and helped to draft the manuscript; JS helped to conceive of the study and participated in the design and interpretation of the data; SEW helped to conceive of the study, participated in the design and interpretation of the data; and supervised the data collection. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ron Pomper.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments and comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 63 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pomper, R., Ellis Weismer, S., Saffran, J. et al. Specificity of Phonological Representations for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 49, 3351–3363 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04054-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04054-5

Keywords

Navigation