Skip to main content
Log in

Visual Dominance and Visual Egalitarianism: Individual and Group-Level Influences of Sex and Status in Group Interactions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated visual dominance and visual egalitarianism of men and women (N = 94; 17 teams) in team meetings at diverse workplaces. Two novel gaze-related measures were developed: (a) a group visual dominance ratio (group-VDR) assessing each member’s visual dominance vis-à-vis all other members, and (b) a gaze distribution index (GDI) assessing each member’s visual egalitarianism to all group members. Multilevel analyses were conducted to account for influences of the team members’ sex and status on the individual level and for influences of sex and status composition of the teams, and the team leaders’ sex on the group level. Results suggested that high-status individuals displayed more visual dominance than low-status individuals. The significant interaction of individuals’ sex and status indicated that the positive relationship of status and visual dominance applied particularly to women. The more women in a team, the more visual dominance was displayed. The team leader’s sex significantly influenced visual egalitarianism: Gaze distribution was less egalitarian when the team leader was male.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. According to Social Role Theory (Eagly 1987) and Role Congruity Theory (Eagly and Karau 2002) women are expected to display higher competence and dominance in feminine tasks, men are expected to display higher competence and dominance in masculine and neutral tasks, particularly in the absence of other diagnostic cues. It is assumed that this expectancy derives from observing men more frequently in higher-status positions than women (Eagly 1987; Eagly and Karau 2002). The findings of Conway et al. (1996) show that low-status persons relative to high-status persons are perceived as more communal and less agentic, and Giannopoulos et al.’s (2005) findings suggest that status may be gendered beyond the relation observed in prior research.

References

  • Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bales, R. F., & Cohen, S. P. (1982). SYMLOG–Ein System für die mehrstufige Beobachtung von Gruppen. [SYMLOG–A system of the multi-layer observation of groups]. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Conner, T. L., & Fisek, M. H. (1974). Expectation states theory: A theoretical research program. Cambridge, MA: Winthrop.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T., & Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality, and agency: Implications for stereotypes of gender and other groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 25–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Copeland, C. L., Driskell, J. E., & Salas, E. (1995). Gender and reactions to dominance. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 53–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 788–827). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Nonverbal communication. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 3–40). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1982). Decoding visual dominance behavior: Attributions of power based on the relative percentages of looking while speaking and looking while listening. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 106–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Pattern of visual dominance behaviors in humans. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance and nonverbal behavior (pp. 129–149). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Brown, C. E., Heltman, K., Ellyson, S. L., & Keating, C. F. (1988a). Power displays between women and men in discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multichannel study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 580–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Ellyson, S. L., Keating, C. F., Heltman, K., & Brown, C. E. (1988b). The relationship of social power to visual displays of dominance between men and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 233–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice towards female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebel, R. L. (1951). Estimation of the reliability of ratings. Psychometrika, 16, 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellyson, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., & Fehr, B. J. (1981). Visual behavior and dominance in women and men. In C. Mayo & N. M. Henley (Eds.), Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 63–79). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V., & Fehr, B. J. (1982). The assessment of gaze and mutual gaze. In P. Ekman & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 91–135). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long, B. (1975). Visual behavior as an aspect of power role relationships. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Alloway (Eds.), Nonverbal communication of aggression (pp. 53–759). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (1999). The continuum model: Ten years later. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 231–254). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giannopoulos, C., Conway, M., & Mendelson, M. (2005). The gender of status: The laypersons’ perception of status groups is gender-typed. Sex Roles, 53, 795–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. London: Hopkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalma, A. (1992). Gazing in triads: A powerful signal in floor apportionment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanter, R. M. (1977). Men and women of the corporation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kestenberg Amighi, J. K., Loman, S., Lewis, P., & Sossin, M. (1999). The meaning of movement: Developmental and clinical perspectives of the Kestenberg Movement Profile. Amsterdam: Gordon and Breach.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kestenberg, J. S., & Sossin, K. M. (1979). The role of movement patterns in development (Vol. 2). New York: Dance Notation Bureau Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (1992). Nonverbal communication in human interaction (3rd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. C. (2004). Constructing gender: A lens-model inspired gender-communication approach. Sex Roles, 51, 171–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. C., & Zumbach, J. (2002). The use of video analysis software in behavior observation research: Interaction patterns in task-oriented small groups. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research [On-line Journal], 3(2). Available at: http://www.qualitative-research.net/fqs/fqs-eng.html.

  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, S. A. (1999). Impacts of diversity and work-life initiatives in organizations. In G. N. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of gender and work (pp. 453–475). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson, M. S. (1997). Theme Software (Online Information). Retrieved Aug 11, 2003 from http://www.hi.is./~msm/behavior.html.

  • Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 32, 93–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1970). A semantic space for nonverbal behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35, 248–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2007). Mplus User`s guide (4th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, G. N. (Ed.). (1999). Handbook of gender and work. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., & Bourg, C. (2004). Gender as status: An expectation states theory approach. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 217–241). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., & Diekema, D. (1992). Are gender differences status differences? In C. L. Ridgeway (Ed.), Gender, interaction, and inequality (pp. 157–180). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., Berger, J., & Smith, L. (1985). Nonverbal cues and status: An expectation states approach. American Journal of Sociology, 90, 955–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M. (2002). Female dominance hierarchies: Are they any different from males’? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 29–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thimm, C. (1990). Dominanz und Sprache. Zur Beschreibung strategischen Handelns im Alltag. [Dominance and language. Strategic behavior in everyday life.]. Wiesbaden, Germany: Deutscher Universitaetsverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thimm, C., Rademacher, U., & Kruse, L. (1994). Power-related talk. Control in verbal interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 328–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoder, J. D. (1991). Rethinking tokenism: Looking beyond numbers. Gender and Society, 5, 178–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was partially supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; KR 505/11). Our gratitude goes particularly to Alice H. Eagly for generous mentoring and repeated revisions of this manuscript. We want to thank Judith Hall for revisions and constructive suggestions for this article, Ursula Hess for editorial revisions, Antje Schroeer for data collection, Barbara Schicht for mathematical support, Marion Lammarsch for technical support, Oliver Schilling for statistical support, and Joseph E. McGrath, Anne Maass, Robyn Cruz, and Sharon Chaiklin for their feedback on early drafts of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sabine C. Koch.

Additional information

The article is dedicated to Joseph E. McGrath who died in April 2007, and to his wife Marion McGrath who followed him in June 2008.

Appendices

Appendix A

Details on the Development of the Group-VDR

Example: For a group of three persons, Formula 2 would be applied as follows (lwt = looks while talking; lwl = looks while listening; gr = group):

$$ {\frac{{{\frac{{{\text{A}}\,{\text{lwt}}\,{\text{B}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{lwt}}\,{\text{C}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{lwt}}\,{\text{gr}}}}{{{\text{A}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{B}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{C}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{gr}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{away}}}}} \times 100}}{{{\frac{{{\text{A}}\,{\text{lwl}}\,{\text{B}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{lwl}}\,{\text{C}} + {\text{A}}\,{\text{lwl}}\,{\text{gr}}}}{{{\text{B}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{A}} + {\text{B}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{C}} + {\text{B}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{gr}} + {\text{B}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{away}} + {\text{C}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{A}} + {\text{C}}\,{\text{talk}} + {\text{B}} + {\text{C}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{gr}} + {\text{C}}\,{\text{talk}}\,{\text{away}}}}} \times 100}}} $$

Missing data due to occasional lack of visibility of some group members and their absence from part of the meeting (e.g., for an incoming phone call) caused us to adjust the group-VDR calculations in two steps: (a) to adjust for lack of visibility, we used the following formula (Formula 4; group-VDR considering out-of-sight times):

$$ {\frac{{{\frac{{{{A}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{total}} - ({{A}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{away}} + {{away}}\,\% \,{{of}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{without}}\,{{sight}})}}{{{{A}}\,{{talk}}\,{{total}}}}} \times 100}}{{{\frac{{{{A}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{total}} - ({{A}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{away}} + {{away}}\,\% \,{{of}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{without}}\,{{sight}})}}{{{{talk}}\,{{total}}\,{{group}}\,{{without}}\,{{A}}}}} \times 100}}}, $$

(b) when out-of-sight times and absence times occurred simultaneously, the following formula provided an adequate solution (Formula 5; group-VDR considering out-of-sight and absence times)

$$ {\frac{{{\frac{{{{A}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{total}} - ({{A}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{away}} + {{away}}\,\% \,{{of}}\,{{lwt}}\,{{without}}\,{{sight}})}}{{{{A}}\,{{talk}}\,{{total}}}}} \times 100}}{{{\frac{{{{A}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{total}} - ({{A}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{away}} + {{away}}\,\% \,{{of}}\,{{lwl}}\,{{without}}\,{{sight}})}}{{{{talk}}\,{{total}}\,{{group}}\,{{without}}\,{{A}} - {{\text{talk}}}\,\% \,{{\text{total}}}\,{{\text{group}}}\,{{\text{in}}}\,{{\text{absence}}}\,{{\text{time}}}({{\text{s}}})}}} \times 100}}} $$

Ad Formula 4: Some persons were sometimes covered by other persons. In these cases we coded the speaking mode (lwt or lwl), yet, the gaze direction, i.e., the target person looked at, needed to be inferred. Presupposing that persons show a similar gaze behavior when they are not visible compared to when they are visible, we chose Formula 4. For example, to calculate ‘away % of lwl without sight’ for person C from team X we proceeded in the following way: person C from team X was in lwl-mode for 14,604 frames. She looked away for 2,715 frames. For 465 frames she was out of sight (oS). The total time that C was in lwl-mode and at the same time visible, thus, amounted to 14,604 − 465 = 11,889 frames. In about 23% of this time she looked away (away = 2,715). Assuming that she would display a similar gaze pattern for the time that she was out-of-sight, we presupposed that she would look away for 23% of the out-of-sight-time as well, which amounted to 106 frames. This value was added to the total observed lwl-away-time and then subtracted from the total listening-time. Lwt-values were treated respectively.

Ad Formula 5: Further modifications in calculating the group-VDR were necessary when person A joined the team meeting at a later point, left at an earlier point, or intermittently left the room, for example, for a phone call. These absence times needed to be taken into account for group-VDR calculations for the absent person in the value ‘talk total group without A’. The assumption was that the lwt-behavior of the whole group during the absence times is proportional to the lwt-behavior of the whole group during the entire observation time. The computations changed as shown in Formula 5.

We calculated the total value of group’s lwt without A in proportion to the entire time of a session in Formula 4. This percent value was the value multiplied with A’s out-of-sight time. The result was the estimator for A’s lwt time while out of sight which was subtracted from the overall values. Hence, the resulting value ‘talk total group without A’ considered the out-of-sight times. When out-of-sight and absence times occurred both at a time Formula 5 provided an adequate solution. Depending on the circumstances encountered, the appropriate formula needs to be chosen (with formula parts in italics flexibly applied).

Technical specifications: A frontal view of all participants allows to compute the group-VDR as precisely as possible. One overall perspective is preferable to split-screen taping, because it increases target accuracy.

Appendix B

Mathematical Derivation of the Gaze Distribution Index (GDI) for a Person p

We calculated the GDI-value of a person as the sum of the absolute differences of this person looking at each other team member and the maximum balanced gaze distribution (i.e., each team member is looked at for the exact same amount of time). The most unbalanced value would result for a person looking at only one person during the entire time. This value, however, is still influenced by group size. In order to get a standardized GDI p -value we calculated the maximum polarized gaze distribution for each possible group size using the Manhattan norm, and divided the empirically derived values by this maximum value for standardization purposes. Resulting GDI p -values were reversed in polarization and lay then between 0 polarized and 1 balanced.

Note: In the following mathematical derivation n = number of persons in the team; t i is the standardized time that the person p looks at another person p i , for i = 1,…, n − 1. (n − 1 being the standardized time)

Each person p is assigned a GDI-value GDI p (i.e., a standardized measure for the gaze distribution of the person p) with the following attributes:

$$ 0 \le {\text{GDI}}_{p} \le 1 $$
  1. a.

    GDI p  = 1 for balanced gaze distribution, i.e., each other person in the team is looked at for the exact same amount of time: \( {\frac{1}{n - 1}} \)

  2. b.

    GDI p  = 0 for maximum polarized gaze distribution, i.e., person p looks at only one person during the entire time;

To obtain a standardized measure of egalitarianism we used the Manhattan norm

$$ \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n - 1} {\left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - t_{i} } \right|} $$

(the sum of the absolute differences of \( {\frac{1}{n - 1}} \) as the balanced gaze distribution and t i the actual standardized gazing time). For maximum balanced gaze results

$$ \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n - 1} {\left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - {\frac{1}{n - 1}}} \right| = 0,} $$

and for maximum polarized gaze, i.e., only one person is gazed at for the entire time:

$$ \left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - 1} \right| + \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n - 2} {\left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - 0} \right| = 1 - } {\frac{1}{n - 1}} + {\frac{n - 2}{n - 1}} = 2 \times {\frac{n - 2}{n - 1}}. $$

A standardized GDI-value (between 0 and 1), fulfilling the conditions (a) to (c), results from

$$ GDI_{p} = 1 - {\frac{{\sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n - 1} {\left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - t_{i} } \right|} }}{{2 \times {\frac{n - 2}{n - 1}}}}} = 1 - {\frac{n - 1}{2(n - 2)}} \times \sum\limits_{i = 1}^{n - 1} {\left| {{\frac{1}{n - 1}} - t_{i} } \right|} $$

After reversal of polarities, GDI-values lie between 0 polarized and 1 balanced.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koch, S.C., Baehne, C.G., Kruse, L. et al. Visual Dominance and Visual Egalitarianism: Individual and Group-Level Influences of Sex and Status in Group Interactions. J Nonverbal Behav 34, 137–153 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-010-0088-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-010-0088-8

Keywords

Navigation