Skip to main content
Log in

More Important, but for What Exactly? The Insignificant Role of Subjective Issue Importance in Vote Decisions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The nature of democratic governance is intimately connected with how citizens respond to candidate position taking. But when will a generally uninformed public base its vote choices on candidate positions? Since Converse scholars have argued that citizens should place greater weight on candidate positions on issues they consider personally important. However, this claim has received mixed empirical support. We revisit this question with compelling new evidence. First, we expand the limited temporal focus of existing work in our first study where we analyze all available ANES data on importance and issue voting between 1980 and 2008. We then overcome endogeneity concerns through a nationally representative conjoint experiment in which we randomize two candidate’s positions on five issues. Results from both studies demonstrate that there is scant evidence that subjective issue importance consistently moderates the relationship between candidate positions and vote choices. We discuss the implications of these results for “issue public” theories of political engagement, for research on voting behavior, and for political representation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Replication materials for this study can be accessed via the Political Behavior Dataverse.

  2. Respondents on the 2008 ANES Time Series were randomly assigned to either Ballot A, where they were asked about seven issues, or to Ballot B where six issues were queried about. We thus investigate the two ballots as if they were two separate surveys, yielding seven ANES Time Series surveys to analyze. Subjective issue importance was not asked on other ANES surveys during this time frame.

  3. This is not the only way of operationalizing issue voting. For instance, voters may alternatively vote directionally and simply favor the candidate that they perceive to be on their side of the issue (e.g. Adams et al. 2004). Our goal is not to adjudicate between these rival theories of decision making. We focus on a proximity rule both because of its prominence in prior studies of issue importance and voting (i.e., Krosnick 1988b) and because Van Houweling and Tomz’ (2008) experimental analyses suggest a proximity rule may be the most commonly used strategy by voters.

  4. See Online Appendix B for full model results. We have also done this on an issue-by-issue basis, which yields 48 models. Our basic conclusions remain the same. Indeed, these issue-by-issue models yield a slightly more pessimistic account. For instance, in only 6/48 cases would a Wald test for the inclusion of importance and the interaction term yield a significant test statistic.

  5. To address this lack of support, we re-estimate interactions using the binning estimator suggested by Hainmueller et al. (2018) and find little reason to believe a simple linear interaction is problematic (see Online Appendix E). Given the small range of possible values of the importance variable, we nearly always have some observations across its full domain mitigating the main concern with interaction analyses about bases inferences on extreme extrapolation.

  6. Such uncertainty may raise questions regarding the measurement of importance and its role in affecting these interactions. Notably, as part of a survey experiment three importance questions on the 2008 Time Series featured an alteration to their response options that led to significantly higher numbers of respondents saying the issues were of low importance. All items on the 2008 ANES Panel, moreover, featured this importance depressing alternative wording. Notably importance continues to play an inconsequential role in moderating issue voting in these cases despite the potential for enhanced precision in estimating the effects of issue distance at lower levels of importance afforded by the change in measurement. In addition, we experimentally induced lower levels of issue importance on one issue in Study 2 yet still did not find a meaningful interaction between importance and issue voting. Finally, in Online Appendix D we address the potential for social desirability to create a skew in the importance measures. We do this by subtracting from each individual importance report the respondent’s mean score on all importance items. Notably we find the same pattern of results as in-text.

  7. The order of this information was constant. As noted, the characteristics of both candidates were fully randomized, i.e. we did not restrict particular combinations of attributes. However, we did restrict the probability with which the race and religion characteristics were drawn to better reflect their population characteristics. The racial categories were drawn randomly with the following probabilities: 0.60 (White), 0.15 (Black), 0.15 (Hispanic) and 0.10 (Asian). The religion categories had the following probabilities of being drawn: 0.20 (Evangelical), 0.20 (Mainline), 0.20 (Catholic), 0.10 (Jewish), 0.10 (Muslim) and 0.20 (None). As a final note, one candidate was always a Democrat and the other a Republican. Which candidate was a Democrat/Republican was randomly assigned.

  8. Our results are the same when we fully stack the data such that each row is a vignette-candidate and proximity is measured as the level of proximity to the candidate in question; see Table OA6 and Figure OA12.

  9. See Figure OA4 for the distribution and sample means for all eight issues.

  10. A similar result emerges on the TPP issue when we use the manipulation treatment assignment instead of importance in the interaction with relative proximity. We also explored a three-way interaction between proximity/importance/and importance manipulation as well as a two-stage least squares equation where we use the exogenous importance treatment condition as an instrument for importance and its interaction with proximity. These analyses also show a failure for importance to positively moderate proximity. See Table OA5/Figure OA10 for these results.

  11. A Wald test for the equivalence of these two effects is not statistically significant [F(1, 655) = 1.27, p = 0.26], i.e. we cannot reject the null that the difference between the two equals 0.

  12. As with above, we cannot reject the null that the two are equivalent; F(1, 657) = 1.84; p = 0.18.

  13. In Online Appendix A we also investigate the relationship between importance, proximity and co-partisanship and find scant evidence that Independents were more likely to behave in accordance with the Issue Public hypothesis. We also compare results taken from just the first vignette versus those obtained from the full sample of vignettes to investigate potential learning effects. Notably, a negative interaction emerged for three of the five issues on the first vignette, which suggests that Fig. 4 is not an artifact of respondent learning over the course of the experiment.

  14. Of course, we are unable to completely rule out the possibility of some projection (that is, respondents viewing party and policy positions of profiles but ignoring or modifying them to some extent.) Researchers employing conjoint experiments on candidate evaluation and vote choice should consider how best to measure this possible projection dynamic.

  15. We thank an Anonymous Reviewer for making this point.

  16. Even if voters choose based on the rational proxy of recent economic performance, as suggested by Fiorina (1981), we may still see such a breakdown occur on non-economic issues, especially when partisan biases in economic perceptions are considered (e.g. Bartels 2002). Of course, there are issues with this type of demand-input model, most importantly the endogeneity of voter preferences to elite rhetoric (Disch 2011; Druckman 2014). When the endogeneity of mass preferences are accounted for the normative implications of proximity voting, of any degree, becomes much more complicated to assess.

References

  • Adams, J., Bishin, B. G., & Dow, J. K. (2004). Representation in congressional campaigns: Evidence for discounting/directional voting in U.S. senate elections. Journal of Politics,66, 348–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ahler, D. J., & Broockman, D. E. (2017). The delegate paradox: Why polarized politicians can represent citizens best. The Journal of Politics. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2958017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aldrich, J. H., & McKelvey, R. D. (1977). A method of scaling with applications to the 1968 and 1972 presidential elections. The American Political Science Review,71(1), 111–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ballard-Rosa, C., Martin, L., & Scheve, K. (2016). The structure of American income tax policy preferences. The Journal of Politics,79(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan Bias in political perceptions. Political Behavior,24(2), 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bélanger, É., & Meguid, B. M. (2008). Issue salience, issue ownership, and issue-based vote choice. Electoral Studies,27, 477–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizer, G. Y., & Krosnick, J. A. (2001). Exploring the structure of strength-related attitude features: The Relation between attitude importance and attitude accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,81(4), 566–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolsen, T., & Leeper, T. J. (2013). Self-Interest and attention to news among issue publics. Political Communication,30(3), 329–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boninger, D. S., Krosnick, J. A., Berent, M. K., & Fabrigar, L. R. (1995). The causes and consequences of attitude importance. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broockman, D. E., & Butler, D. M. (2017). The causal effects of elite position-taking on voter attitudes: Field experiments with elite communication. American Journal of Political Science,61(1), 208–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burden, B. C., & Sanberg, J. N. R. (2003). Budget rhetoric in presidential campaigns from 1952 to 2000. Political Behavior,25(2), 97–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmines, Edward G., & Stimson, James A. (1980). The two faces of issue voting. The American Political Science Review,74(1), 78–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnes, N., & Lupu, N. (2016). Do voters dislike politicians from the working class? American Political Science Review,110(4), 832–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpini, M. X. D., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, R. A. (1971). Polyarchy: Participation and opposition. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Disch, L. (2011). Toward a mobilization conception of democratic representation. American Political Science Review,105(1), 100–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. Boston: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2014). Pathologies of studying public opinion, political communication, and democratic responsiveness. Political Communication,31(3), 467–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fournier, P., Blais, A., Nadeau, R., Gidengil, E., & Nevitte, N. (2003). Issue importance and performance voting. Political Behavior,25(1), 51–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granberg, D., & Holmberg, S. (1986). Political perception among voters in Sweden and the U.S.: Analyses of issues with explicit alternatives. The Western Political Quarterly,39(1), 7–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grynaviski, J. D., & Corrigan, B. E. (2006). Specification issues in proximity models of candidate evaluation (with issue importance). Political Analysis,14, 393–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Hangartner, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,112(8), 2395–2400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., & Hopkins, D. J. (2015). The hidden American immigration consensus: Analysis of attitudes toward immigrants. American Journal of Political Science,59(3), 529–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis,22(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Mummolo, J., & Xu, Y. (2018). How much should we trust estimates from multiplicative interaction models? Simple tools to improve empirical practice. Political Analysis.

  • Hanretty, C., Lauderdale, B. E., & Vivyan N. (2018). Measuring Issue Importance.

  • Henderson, M. (2014). Issue publics, campaigns, and political knowledge. Political Behavior,36(3), 631–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinckley, B., Hofstetter, R., & Kessel, J. (1974). Information and the vote: A comparative election study. American Politics Quarterly,2(2), 131–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Krosnick, J. A., Visser, P. S., & Boninger, D. S. (2005). Attitude importance and the accumulation of attitude-relevant knowledge in memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58(5), 749–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holbrook, A. L., Sterrett, D., Johnson, T. P., & Krysan, M. (2016). Racial disparities in political participation across issues: The role of issue-specific motivators. Political Behavior,38(1), 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, P. A., & Hill, K. Q. (2003). Beyond the demand-input model: A theory of representational linkages. The Journal of Politics,65(2), 304–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchings, V. L. (2001). Political context, issue salience, and selective attentiveness: Constituent knowledge of the Clarence Thomas confirmation vote. The Journal of Politics,63(3), 846–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. (1990). Shortcuts to political knowledge: The role of selective attention and accessibility. In information and democratic processes. (pp. 160–195).

  • Iyengar, S., Hahn, K. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Walker, J. (2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. The Journal of Politics,70(1), 186–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. E. (1980). Analysis of pilot study issue questions. ANES Pilot Study Report (No. nes002236), 1–42. http://www.electionstudies.org/Library/papers/documents/nes002236.pdf.

  • Johns, R. (2010). Measuring issue salience in british elections: Competing interpretations of ‘most important issue’. Political Research Quarterly,63(1), 143–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkland, P. A., & Coppock, A. (2017). Candidate choice without party labels: New insights from conjoint survey experiments. Political Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9414-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. (1988a). Attitude importance and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,24, 240–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. (1988b). The role of attitude importance in social evaluation: A study of policy preferences, presidential candidate evaluations, and voting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,55(2), 196–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A. 1990. Government policy and citizen passion: A study of issue publics in contemporary America. Political Behavior, 12(1), 59–92. http://www.jstor.org/stable/586285.

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How voters decide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, H., Borgida, E., & Sullivan, J. L. (2000). On the relationship between attitude involvement and attitude accessibility: Toward a cognitive-motivational model of political information processing. Political Psychology,21(1), 81–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine, H. G., Johnston, C. D., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2012). The ambivalent partisan: How critical loyalty promotes democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lecheler, S., de Vreese, C., & Slothuus, R. (2009). Issue importance as a moderator of framing effects. Communication Research,36(3), 400–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T. J. (2014). The informational basis for mass polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly,78(1), 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the priming hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science,53(4), 821–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenz, G. S. (2012). Follow the leader? How voters respond to politicians’ policies and performance. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maggiotto, M. A., & Piereson, J. E. (1978). Issue publics and voter choice. American Politics Quarterly,6, 407–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. E., & MacKuen, M. B. (1993). Anxiety, enthusiasm, and the vote: The emotional underpinnings of learning and involvement during presidential campaigns. The American Political Science Review, 87(3), 672–685. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2938743.

  • Miller, J. M., Krosnick, J. A., & Fabrigar, L. R. (2017). The origins of policy issue salience: Personal and national importance impact on behavioral, cognitive, and emotional issue engagement. In J. A. Krosnick, I. C. A. Chiang, & T. H. Stark (Eds.), Political psychology: New explorations. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. M., & Peterson, D. A. M. (2004). Theoretical and empirical implications of attitude strength. The Journal of Politics,66(3), 847–867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niemi, R. G., & Bartels, L. M. (1985). New measures on issue salience: An evaluation. The Journal of Politics,47(4), 1212–1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page, B. I., & Brody, R. A. (1972). Policy voting and the electoral process: the Vietnam war issue. The American Political Science Review,66(3), 979–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, E. (2017). The role of the information environment in partisan voting. The Journal of Politics,79(4), 1191–1204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petrocik, J. R., Benoit, W. L., & Hansen, G. J. (2003). Issue ownership and presidential campaigning, 1952–2000. Political Science Quarterly,118(4), 599–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2017). Americans express increasingly warm feelings toward religious groups.

  • Price, V., David, C., Goldthorpe, B., Roth, M. M., & Cappella, J. N. (2006). Locating the issue public: The multi-dimensional nature of engagement with health care reform. Political Behavior,28(1), 33–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rabinowitz, G., Prothro, J. W., & Jacoby, W. (1982). Salience as a factor in the impact of issues on candidate evaluation. The Journal of Politics,44(1), 41–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spoon, J. J., & Klüver, H. (2014). Do parties respond? How electoral context influences party responsiveness. Electoral Studies,35, 48–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomz, M., & Van Houweling, R. P. (2008). Candidate positioning and voter choice. The American Political Science Review,102(3), 303–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Simmons, J. P. (2003). Distinguishing the cognitive and behavioral consequences of attitude importance and certainty: A new approach to testing the common-factor hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,39, 118–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

An earlier version of this manuscript was presented at the 2016 Annual conference of the American Political Science Association. We thank Scott Clifford and Rune Stubager for their insightful comments on the manuscript and the Department of Political Science at Aarhus University for research support for Study 2. Author order is alphabetical. All errors remain our own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joshua Robison.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 1698 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leeper, T.J., Robison, J. More Important, but for What Exactly? The Insignificant Role of Subjective Issue Importance in Vote Decisions. Polit Behav 42, 239–259 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9494-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9494-0

Keywords

Navigation