Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Contribution of Naming Speed to the Simple View of Reading

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate whether naming speed makes a contribution to the prediction of reading comprehension, after taking into account the product of word decoding and listening comprehension (i.e., the Simple View of Reading; [Gough, P.B. & Tunmer, W.E. (1986). Remedial and Special Education 7, 6–10]), and phonological awareness. In grade 3, word decoding was measured with the Woodcock [(1998). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests – Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services]. Word Identification and Word Attack subtests, listening comprehension with the Woodcock (1991) [Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery – Revised. Chicago: Riverside Publishing Company] test of Listening Comprehension, naming speed with a picture naming task, and 4 measures assessed phonological awareness. Reading comprehension was assessed in grades 3, 4, and 5 with the Woodcock (1998) Passage Comprehension subtest and in grade 5 with the Gates–MacGinitie reading test. The Simple View was evaluated twice: first, with a pseudoword measure for decoding (Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product) and, second, with a word identification measure for decoding (word recognition product). Hierarchical regression and commonality analyses indicated that the decoding and listening comprehension products accounted for considerable variance in reading comprehension. Naming speed had a small but significant effect after accounting for the Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product (2–3%), but little effect after accounting for the word-recognition product (0–2%). Subgroup analyses indicated that naming speed had its primary effect for less able readers. Commonality analyses supported the interpretation that naming speed contributes after the Grapheme–Phoneme-conversion product but not after the word recognition product because naming speed has already had its effect upon word recognition. These results indicate that it is important how the Simple View decoding term is defined, and that the Simple View may be incomplete, especially for less able readers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • M. J. Adams (1990) Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print MIT Press Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • P. G. Bowers E. Newby-Clark (2002) ArticleTitleThe role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 15 109–126 Occurrence Handle10.1023/A:1013820421199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • P. E. Bryant (1985) Children’s reading problems Blackwell Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • R. P. Carver (1998) ArticleTitlePredicting reading level in grades 1 to 6 from listening level and decoding level: Testing theory relevant to the simple view of reading Reading, Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 10 121–154 Occurrence Handle10.1023/A:1007923124312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R. P. Carver (2000) The causes of high and low reading achievement Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • L. G. Dreyer L. Katz (1992) ArticleTitleAn examination of “The simple view of reading” National Reading Conference Yearbook 41 169–175

    Google Scholar 

  • L. C. Ehri (1998) Grapheme–phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English J.L. Metsala Ehri (Eds) Word recognition in beginning literacy Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah NJ 3–40

    Google Scholar 

  • P. B. Gough W. A. Hoover C. L. Peterson (1996) Some observations on the simple view of reading A. Cornoldi J. Oakhill (Eds) Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah NJ 1–13

    Google Scholar 

  • P. B. Gough W. E. Tunmer (1986) ArticleTitleDecoding, reading, and reading disability Remedial and Special Education 7 6–10 Occurrence Handle10.1177/074193258600700104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • W. A. Hoover P. B. Gough (1990) ArticleTitleThe simple view of reading Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2 127–160 Occurrence Handle10.1007/BF00401799

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R. M. Joshi P. G. Aaron (2000) ArticleTitleThe component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex Reading Psychology 21 85–97 Occurrence Handle10.1080/02702710050084428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • J. R. Kirby R. K. Parrila S. L. Pfeiffer (2003) ArticleTitleNaming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development Journal of Educational Psychology 95 453–464 Occurrence Handle10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • T. R. Konold C. Juel M. McKinnon R. Deffes (2003) ArticleTitleA multivariate model of early reading acquisition Applied Psycholinguistics 24 89–112 Occurrence Handle10.1017/S0142716403000055

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D. L. Lefly B. F. Pennington (1991) ArticleTitleSpelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics Annals of Dyslexia 41 143–162

    Google Scholar 

  • W. H. MacGinitie R. K. MacGinitie (1992) Gates–MacGinitie reading tests EditionNumber2nd Canadian Nelson Toronto

    Google Scholar 

  • F. R. Manis L. M. Doi B. Bhadha (2000) ArticleTitleNaming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders Journal of Learning Disabilities 33 325–333

    Google Scholar 

  • C. McBride-Chang F. R. Manis (1996) ArticleTitleStructural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8 323–339 Occurrence Handle10.1007/BF00395112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • M. S. Meyer F. B. Wood L. A. Hart R. H. Felton (1998a) ArticleTitleLongitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers Annals of Dyslexia 48 91–114

    Google Scholar 

  • M. S. Meyer F. B. Wood L. A. Hart R. H. Felton (1998b) ArticleTitleSelective predictive value of rapid automatized naming in poor readers Journal of Learning Disabilities 31 106–117 Occurrence Handle10.1177/002221949803100201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • K. Nation (1999) ArticleTitleReading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective Psychological Bulletin 125 338–355 Occurrence Handle10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • E. J. Pedhazur (1982) Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction EditionNumber2 Holt, Rinehart and Winston New York

    Google Scholar 

  • R. Savage (2001) ArticleTitleThe ‘simple view’ of reading: Some evidence and possible implications Educational Psychology in Practice 17 17–33

    Google Scholar 

  • H. S. Scarborough (1998a) Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors B.K. Shapiro P. J. Accardo A. J. Capute (Eds) Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum York Press Timonium MD 75–119

    Google Scholar 

  • H. S. Scarborough (1998b) ArticleTitlePredicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ Annals of Dyslexia 48 115–136

    Google Scholar 

  • R. K. Wagner J. K. Torgesen P. L. Laughon K. Simmons C. Rashotte (1993) ArticleTitleDevelopment of young readers’ phonological processing abilities Journal of Educational Psychology 85 83–103 Occurrence Handle10.1037/0022-0663.85.1.83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • M. H. Wolf Bally R. Morris (1986) ArticleTitleAutomaticity, retrieval processes and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers Child Development 57 988–1000

    Google Scholar 

  • M. Wolf P. G. Bowers (1999) ArticleTitleThe double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias Journal of Educational Psychology 91 415–438 Occurrence Handle10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • M. Wolf T. Katzir-Cohen (2001) ArticleTitleReading fluency and its intervention Scientific Studies of Reading 5 211–238 Occurrence Handle10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R. Woodcock (1991) Woodcock language proficiency battery – Revised Riverside Publishing Company Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • R. Woodcock (1998) Woodcock reading mastery tests – Revised American Guidance Services Circle Pines, MN

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John R. Kirby.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Johnston, T.C., Kirby, J.R. The Contribution of Naming Speed to the Simple View of Reading. Read Writ 19, 339–361 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-4644-2

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-4644-2

Keywords

Navigation