Skip to main content
Log in

Theories of Attachment: The Long and Winding Road to an Integrative Developmental Science

  • Commentary
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although traditional accounts of attachment theory attempted to partition the organism’s attachment and separation responses into those that were instinctive and those that were the result of the developmental environment, recent findings from epigenetics are indicating that no such partitioning is possible, even in principle. Rather than assuming the expression of a given behavioral trait is based on some set of instincts (as Bowlby and many of his colleagues did for attachment and separation responses), behavioral development is now seen as a self-organizing, probabilistic process in which pattern and order emerge and change as a result of ongoing co-actions among developmentally relevant components both internal (e.g., genes, hormones, neural networks) and external (e.g., temperature, diet, social interaction) to the organism. Exploring the specific prenatal and postnatal features of the mother–infant interaction system is providing a new appreciation of the complexity of the origins and maintenance of early attachment and its long-term consequences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Lachmann, F., Feldstein, S., Crown, C., & Jasnow, J. (2000). Systems models in development and psychoanalysis: the case of vocal rhythm coordination and attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 21, 99–122. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(200001/04)21:1/2<99::AID-IMHJ11>3.0.CO;2-#.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., et al. (2008). Microanalysis of mother–infant interaction at 4-months predicts 12-month attachment. Attachment and Human Development (in press).

  • Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. The International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 33, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, vol. 1. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, R. B. (1998). The making of developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol 1: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 25–106). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feng, J., Fouse, S., & Fan, G. (2007). Epigenetic regulation of neural gene expression and neural function. Pediatric Research, 61, 58R–63R. doi:10.1203/pdr.0b013e3180457635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fish, E. W., Shahrokh, D., Bagot, R., Caldji, C., Bredy, T., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epepigenetic programming of stress responses through variations in maternal care. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036, 167–180. doi:10.1196/annals.1330.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, N., & Card, J. A. (1999). Psychophysiological measures in the study of attachment. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M .J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations in maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 1155– 1158.

  • Gottlieb, G. (1971). Development of Species identification in birds. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature–nurture: Prenatal origins of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G., & Lickliter, R. (2004). The various roles of animal models in understanding human development. Social Development, 13, 311–325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.000269.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, M. M., & Maples, E. G. (1998). Species-typical behavior. In Comparative psychology: A handbook (pp. 191–197). New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (1994). Early relationships as regulators of infant physiology and behavior. Acta Paediatrica (Oslo, Norway), 397, 9–18. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13260.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (2006). Psychobiological roots of early attachment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 84–88. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00412.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Pynn, J., Fragaszy, D. M., & Cummin-Sebree, S. (2003). Common territories in comparative and developmental psychology: quest for shared means and meanings in behavioral investigations. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. D., & Edwards, L. (2002). Genes, interactions, and development. Psychological Review, 109, 26–34. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. D., & Gottlieb, G. (1981). Development of visual species identification in ducklings: what is the role of imprinting? Animal Behaviour, 29, 1082–1099. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80061-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, J. M., & Sweatt, J. D. (2005). Epigenetic mechanisms in memory formation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 6, 108–118. doi:10.1038/nrn1604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R. (2005). Prenatal sensory ecology and experience: implications for perceptual and behavioral development in precocial birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 235–274. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(05)35006-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Bahrick, L. E. (2000). The development of infant intersensory perception: advantages of a comparative convergent-operations approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 260–280. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Bahrick, L. E. (2007). Thinking about development: the value of animal-based research for the study of human development. European Journal of Developmental Science, 1, 172–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Gottlieb, G. (1985). Social interaction with siblings is necessary for the visual imprinting of species-specific maternal preference in ducklings. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 371–378. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.99.4.371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1937). The companion in the bird’s world. The Auk, 54, 245–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134, 1501–1506. doi:10.1126/science.134.3489.1501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michel, G., & Moore, C. (1995). Developmental psychobiology: An integrative science. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. B. (1997). The effects of nonobvious forms of experience on the development of instinctive behavior. In C. Dent-Reed, & P. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development (pp. 457–507). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Moffit, T. E. (2005). The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: gene–environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 533–554. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polan, H. J., & Hofer, M. A. (1999). Psychobiological origins of infant attachment and separation responses. In J. Cassidy, & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 162–180). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions: retrospect and prospect. Journal of Gerontology, 60B, 65–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, C. G., Diorio, J., Seckl, M. R., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: characterization of intracellular mediators and potential genomic targets. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1024, 182–212. doi:10.1196/annals.1321.099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, M. J., King, A. P., & White, D. J. (2003). The case for developmental ecology. Animal Behaviour, 66, 617–622. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Lickliter.

Additional information

The writing of this commentary was supported by NICHD grant RO1 HD048432. I thank Lorraine Bahrick for constructive comments on a draft of the commentary.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lickliter, R. Theories of Attachment: The Long and Winding Road to an Integrative Developmental Science. Integr. psych. behav. 42, 397–405 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9073-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9073-8

Keywords

Navigation