Skip to main content
Log in

The Cognitive Bases of Anthropomorphism: From Relatedness to Empathy

  • Published:
International Journal of Social Robotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Humans may react very differently with respect to mechanical devices, including robots. They can interact with them with delight or retreat in aversion or fear. According to the famous model of the uncanny valley these opposite reactions depend on the degree of familiarity that different artifacts engender in humans. The aim of my work is trying to find out the cognitive bases of familiarity, analyzing the origin of anthropomorphic projection, namely human disposition to attribute anthropomorphic features - like intentions or feelings—to artifacts. I shall discuss two concepts: relatedness and empathy, and argue that relatedness is the precondition for empathy. The fact that it is possible to attribute anthropomorphic features virtually to any object shows that resemblance is not the point. Anthropomorphism is a kind of relation that humans establish with an artifact, and in order to comprehend this phenomenon we have to focus on the relational aspect. I shall argue that what we call anthropomorphism is an extension to nonhumans of forms of interactions typical of human communication, i.e. the attribution to an artifact of the position of interlocutor in a possible dialogue. It can be shown that attributing to an artifact the position of interlocutor in a dialogue implies dealing with it as if it were endowed of the features characterizing human mind, i.e. mental states and emotions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I consider that Chella and Manzotti in this article give a clear presentation of the theoretical issues involved in modeling machine consciousness, even if they take a position different from mine arguing against “biological chauvinism”.

  2. Since its original publication in 1970, Mori’s work raised an intense debate. Criticisms pointed to the fact that the uncanny valley was a hypothesis that needs to be validated. Nonetheless, it proved productive and was applied to different areas of research [24]. More recently, a number of psychological studies have undertaken the empirical evaluation of the graph proposed by Mori (see for instance, [2527]). It is impossible to make here a synthesis of the results. To summarize we can assert that what emerges is an articulated set of phenomena that changes and enriches the concept of uncanny valley without disavowing it.

  3. In his later work Piaget took a different position, maintaining that children actually are able to distinguish people from physical objects because they react to the child [30].

  4. It is interesting to note that this area of studies had its origins in ethology. At the beginning the question was about the representations that chimpanzees had of the humans with whom they interacted [36].

  5. In these studies the mother and the infant were in two separate rooms and they interacted viewing each other in a life-sized video image immediately before them. After some minutes of normal interaction the communication was perturbed showing to the infant mother’s behaviors that occurred in a previous time and were not correlated with the present infant’s behavior. While during live communication the infant behaved as in normal face-to-face interactions, in the replay phase the reaction of the infant was one of distress. In another condition it was the mother who unknowingly was presented with her infant’s reaction to her previous behavior and then unrelated with her current one. Several mothers remarked that the interaction was odd and all of them changed their communication focusing more on their own experience than on the infants’ one.

References

  1. Singer T (2006) The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of the literature and implication for future research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:855–863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Thompson RA (1987) Empathy and emotional understanding: the early development of empathy. In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J (eds) Empathy and its development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 119–145

    Google Scholar 

  3. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognit Brain Res 3:131–141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty JP, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2004) Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303:1157–1162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Jackson PL, Rainville P, Decety J (2006) To what extent do we share the pain of others? Insight from neural bases of pain empathy. Pain 125:5–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cognit Sci 8:396–403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty JP, Klaas ES, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2006) Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439:466–469

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Lamm K, Batson CD, Decety J (2007) The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cognit Neurosci 19:42–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Decety J (2010) To what extent is the experience of empathy mediated by neural shared circuits? Emot Rev 2:204–207

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. De Vignemont F, Singer T (2006) The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cognit Sci 10:435–441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kahn PH Jr, Ishiguro H, Friedman B, Kanda T, Freier NG, Severson RL, Miller J (2007) What is a human? Toward psychological benchmarks in the field of human-robot interaction. Interact Stud 8(3):363–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Scassellati B (2002) Theory of mind for a humanoid robot. Auton Robots 12:13–24

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  13. Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42:335–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Searle JR (1980) Minds, brains and programs. Behav Brain Sci 3:417–424

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Searle JR (1990) Consciousness, explanatory inversion, and cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 13:585–596

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Searle JR (1992) The rediscovery of the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  17. Searle JR (1997) The mystery of consciousness. The New York Review of Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  18. Chella A, Manzotti R (2009) Machine consciousness: a manifesto for robotics. Int J Mach Conscious 1:33–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Pfeifer R, Lungarella M, Iida F (2007) Self-organization, embodiment, and biologically inspired robotics. Science 318:1088–1093

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ziemke T (2008) On the role of emotion in biological and robotic autonomy. BioSystems 91:401–408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body, emotion and the making of consciousness. Vintage, London

    Google Scholar 

  22. Arbib MA, Fellous J-M (2004) Emotions: from brain to robot. Trends Cognit Sci 8(12):554–561

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Mori M (1970) The uncanny valley. Energy 7(4):33–35

    Google Scholar 

  24. Gee FC, Browne WN, Kawamura K (2005) Uncanny valley revised. In: IEEE international workshop on robots and human interactive communication, vol 2005, pp 151–157

  25. Tinwell A, Grimshaw M, Nabi DA, Williams A (2011) Facial expression of emotion and perception of the Uncanny Valley in virtual characters. Comput Hum Behav 27:741–749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Saygin AP, Chaminade T, Ishiguro H, Driver J, Frith C (2012) The thing that should not be: predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving human and humanoid robots actions. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 7:413–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Piwek L, McKay LS, Pollick FE (2014) Empirical evaluation of the uncanny valley hypothesis fails to confirm the predicted effect of motion. Cognition 130:271–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Piaget J (1945) La Formation du symbole chez l’enfant : imitation, jeu et rêve, image et représentation. Neuchâtel, Delachaux et Niestlé

  29. Piaget J (1926) La Représentation du monde chez l’enfant. Alcan, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  30. Piaget J (1995) Sociological studies. Routledge, London (Orginal work published in 1977)

  31. Wellman HM, Estes D (1986) Early understanding of mental entities: a reexamination of childhood realism. Child Dev 57:910–923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Samuels A, Taylor M (1994) Children’s ability to distinguish fantasy events from real-life events. Br J Dev Psychol 12:417–427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Lillard AS (1994) Making sense of pretence. In: Lewis C, Mitchell P (eds) Children’s early understanding of mind: origins and development. Erlbaum, Hove, pp 211–234

    Google Scholar 

  34. Woolley JD (1997) Thinking about fantasy: are children fundamentally different thinkers and believers from adults? Child Dev 68:991–1011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Harris PL (2000) The work of the imagination. Basil Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  36. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does a chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Woodward AL (1999) Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behav Dev 22:145–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Behne T, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2005) Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Dev Psychol 41:328–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Premack D (1990) The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition 36:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Spelke ES (1990) Principles of object perception. Cognit Sci 14:29–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Gergely G, Nadasdy Z, Csibra G, Biro S (1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56:165–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Gergely G, Csibra G (2003) Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naive theory of rational action. Trends Cognit Sci 7:287–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Premack D, James Premack A (1997) Motor competence as integral to attribution of goal. Cognition 63:235–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2007) Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450:557–560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2010) 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Dev Sci 13:923–929

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Bühler K (1999) The Mental Development of the child. Routledge, London (Orginal work published in 1930)

  47. Bräten S (ed) (1998) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

  48. Bateson MC (1979) The epigenesis of conversational interaction: a personal account of research development. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech. the beginning of human communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 63–67

    Google Scholar 

  49. Trevarthen C (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech. the beginning of human communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 321–347

    Google Scholar 

  50. Nadel J, Butterworth G (eds) (1999) Imitation in infancy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

  51. Airenti G (2010) Is a naturalistic theory of communication possible? Cognit Syst Res 11:165–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Reddy V (2008) How infants know minds. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  53. Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture, and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic Press, London, pp 183–229

    Google Scholar 

  54. Meltzoff AN, Brooks R, Shon AP, Rao RPN (2010) “Social” robots are psychological agents for infants: a test of gaze following. Neural Netw 23:966–972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Campos JJ, Stenberg CR (1981) Perception, appraisal and emotion: the onset of social referencing. In: Lamb ME, Sherrod LR (eds) Infant social cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 273–314

    Google Scholar 

  56. Feinman S (1982) Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quart 28:445–470

    Google Scholar 

  57. Weizenbaum J (1966) ELIZA—a computer program for the study of natural language communication between man and machine. Commun ACM 9:36–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Stephan A (2013) Empathy for artificial agents. Paper presented at the third joint IEEE international conference on development and learning and on epigenetic robotic. Special session “Constructing empathy: an interdisciplinary investigation”, Osaka

  59. Gray K, Wegner DM (2012) Feeling robots and human zombies: mind perception and the uncanny valley. Cognition 125:125–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Duffy BR (2003) Anthropomorphism and the social robot. Robotics Auton Syst 42:177–190

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  61. Gazzola V, Rizzolatti G, Wicker B, Keysers C (2007) The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. NeuroImage 35:1674–1684

  62. Waytz A, Cacioppo J, Epley N (2010) Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Perspect Psychol Sci 5:219–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Kaplan F (2004) Who is afraid of the humanoid? Investigating cultural differences in the acceptance of robots. Int J Humanoid Robotics 1(3):1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Murray L, Trevarthen C (1985) Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In: Field T, Fox N (eds) Social perception in infants. Ablex, Norwood, pp 177–197

    Google Scholar 

  65. Murray L, Trevarthen C (1986) The infant’s role in mother-infant communication. J Child Lang 13:15–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Murray L (1998) Contributions of experimental and clinical perturbations of mother-infant communication to the understanding of infant intersubjectivity. In: Bråten S (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 127–143

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gabriella Airenti.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Airenti, G. The Cognitive Bases of Anthropomorphism: From Relatedness to Empathy. Int J of Soc Robotics 7, 117–127 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-014-0263-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-014-0263-x

Keywords

Navigation