Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

At the Neural Intersection Between Language and Emotion

  • REVIEW
  • Published:
Affective Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What role does language play in emotion? Behavioral research shows that emotion words such as “anger” and “fear” alter emotion experience, but questions still remain about mechanism. Here, we review the neuroscience literature to examine whether neural processes associated with semantics are also involved in emotion. Our review suggests that brain regions involved in the semantic processing of words: (i) are engaged during experiences of emotion, (ii) coordinate with brain regions involved in affect to create emotions, (iii) hold representational content for emotion, and (iv) may be necessary for constructing emotional experience. We relate these findings with respect to four theoretical relationships between language and emotion, which we refer to as “non-interactive,” “interactive,” “constitutive,” and “deterministic.” We conclude that findings are most consistent with the interactive and constitutive views with initial evidence suggestive of a constitutive view, in particular. We close with several future directions that may help test hypotheses of the constitutive view.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Notably, the distinction we draw between interactive and constitutive roles parallels Aristotelian efficient and material causality, respectively. This distinction is a common theme in emotion research. When considering the relationship between emotion and appraisal, Ellsworth (2013) points out that the question of whether appraisals “cause” emotion is vague. Appraisals can be viewed as causing emotion in the sense that they are separate from emotion and trigger emotions (e.g., as one pool ball causes another to move; an interactive view) or—as she proposes—in the sense that they are part of the emotion (i.e., appraisals are an ingredient of the emotion; a constitutive view). The same ambiguity arises when considering the relationship between emotion and cognition more broadly. For instance, Pessoa (2008) advocates for a constitutive relationship—that the same underlying and overlapping processes compose emotion and cognition and thus the same domain-general ingredients make emotions and cognitions (also see Barrett & Satpute, 2013; Wager et al., 2015). And yet a more commonplace assumption is that emotion “interacts with” cognition in the sense that one exerts an influence upon the other or vice versa. This distinction is usefully applied to areas outside of emotion as well. For instance, in cognitive psychology, ongoing debates examine whether concepts are represented apart from sensory-motor representations but may interact with them (i.e., an interactive view), or whether concepts are constituted from these sensory-motor representations themselves (i.e., a constitutive view, see Barsalou, 2008; Binder, 2016; Deacon, 1998; Fernandino et al., 2016; Leshinskaya & Caramazza, 2016). And parallel arguments have occurred in language and thought, in which many researchers may agree with the more general claim that “language shapes thought,” but the disagreement and potential for theoretical advances lies in whether language is constitutive of thought (indeed is the vehicle for thought), or whether language merely interacts with thought (Boroditsky, 2001; Kay & Kempton, 1984). In general, whether psychological constructs relate with one another interactively or constitutively is a difficult, yet important theme in psychology and neuroscience.

References

  • Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 167–218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S., Gendron, M., Mize, J., & Brennan, L. (2007). Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 2, 297–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Language as context for the perception of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 327–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361–372.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2019). Historical pitfalls and new directions in the neuroscience of emotion. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 9–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bertoux, M., Duclos, H., Caillaud, M., Segobin, S., Merck, C., de la Sayette, V., Belliard, S., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., & Laisney, M. (2020). When affect overlaps with concept: Emotion recognition in semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Brain - A Journal of Neurology, awaa313.

  • Binder, J. R. (2016). In defense of abstract conceptual representations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1096–1108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words as social tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borod, J. C., Koff, E., Lorch, M. P., & Nicholas, M. (1986). The expression and perception of facial emotion in brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 24, 169–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2011). How language shapes thought. Scientific American, 304, 62–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brambati, S., Rankin, K., Narvid, J., Seeley, W., Dean, D., Rosen, H., Miller, B. L., Ashburner, J., & Gorno-Tempini, M. (2009). Atrophy progression in semantic dementia with asymmetric temporal involvement: A tensor-based morphometry study. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 103–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, J. A., Shablack, H., Gendron, M., Satpute, A. B., Parrish, M. H., & Lindquist, K. A. (2016). The role of language in the experience and perception of emotion: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 169–183.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buhle, J. T., Kober, H., Ochsner, K. N., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Weber, J., Hughes, B. L., Kross, E., Atlas, L. Y., McRae, K., & Wager, T. D. (2012). Common representation of pain and negative emotion in the midbrain periaqueductal gray. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 609–616.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Campanella, F., Shallice, T., Ius, T., Fabbro, F., & Skrap, M. (2014). Impact of brain tumour location on emotion and personality: A voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping study on mentalization processes. Brain, 137, 2532–2545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicone, M., Wapner, W., & Gardner, H. (1980). Sensitivity to emotional expressions and situations in organic patients. Cortex, 16, 145–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark-Polner, E., Wager, T. D., Satpute, A. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). The brain basis of affect, emotion, and emotion regulation: Current issues. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 146–165). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2008). Appraisal theories: How cognition shapes affect into emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (p. 628–642). The Guilford Press.

  • Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2013). Psychological construction in the OCC model of emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 335–343.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2009). Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1933–1942.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. D. (2009). Psychophysiology of neural, cognitive and affective integration: fMRI and autonomic indicants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73, 88–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A., & Stillman, P. E. (2013). Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review, 5, 344–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T. (2004). The emotional brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 583–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 143–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1998). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain: WW Norton & Company.

  • Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3, 364–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C. (2013). Appraisal theory: Old and new questions. Emotion Review, 5, 125–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Series in affective science. Handbook of affective sciences (p. 572–595). Oxford University Press.

  • Ethofer, T., Van De Ville, D., Scherer, K., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Decoding of emotional information in voice-sensitive cortices. Current Biology, 19, 1028–1033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandino, L., Humphries, C. J., Conant, L. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Binder, J. R. (2016). Heteromodal cortical areas encode sensory-motor features of word meaning. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 9763–9769.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, J. J. R., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (Eds.). (2013). Series in affective science. Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford University Press.

  • Gendron, M., Hoemann, K., Crittenden, A. N., Mangola, S. M., Ruark, G. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2020). Emotion perception in Hadza Hunter-Gatherers. Scientific Reports, 10, 3867.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Hillis, A. E., Weintraub, S., Kertesz, A., Mendez, M., Cappa, S. F., Ogar, J. M., Rohrer, J. D., Black, S., Boeve, B. F., & Manes, F. (2011). Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology, 76, 1006–1014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grossi, D., Di Vita, A., Palermo, L., Sabatini, U., Trojano, L., & Guariglia, C. (2014). The brain network for self-feeling: A symptom-lesion mapping study. Neuropsychologia, 63, 92–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., McMillan, C., Moore, P., Ding, L., Glosser, G., Work, M., & Gee, J. (2004). What’s in a name: Voxel-based morphometric analyses of MRI and naming difficulty in Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal dementia and corticobasal degeneration. Brain, 127, 628–649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guillory, S. A., & Bujarski, K. A. (2014). Exploring emotions using invasive methods: Review of 60 years of human intracranial electrophysiology. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1880–1889.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haley, K. L., Womack, J. L., Harmon, T. G., & Williams, S. W. (2015). Visual analog rating of mood by people with aphasia. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 22, 239–245.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, S. (2012). Mapping discrete and dimensional emotions onto the brain: Controversies and consensus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 458–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport, 11, 43–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2007). Semantic dementia: A unique clinicopathological syndrome. The Lancet Neurology, 6, 1004–1014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, P., Jefferies, B., & Ralph, M. L. (2015). Special issue of Neuropsychologia: Semantic cognition. Neuropsychologia, 76, 1–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. C., Watts, J., Henry, T. R., List, J.-M., Forkel, R., Mucha, P. J., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., & Lindquist, K. A. (2019). Emotion semantics show both cultural variation and universal structure. Science, 366(6472), 1517–1522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, R. L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2016). The semantic network at work and rest: Differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 1490–1501.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jastorff, J., De Winter, F. L., Van den Stock, J., Vandenberghe, R., Giese, M. A., & Vandenbulcke, M. (2016). Functional dissociation between anterior temporal lobe and inferior frontal gyrus in the processing of dynamic body expressions: Insights from behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 4472–4486.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kassam, K. S., Markey, A. R., Cherkassky, V. L., Loewenstein, G., & Just, M. A. (2013). Identifying emotions on the basis of neural activation. PLoS One, 8, e66032.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkland, T., & Cunningham, W. A. (2012). Mapping emotions through time: How affective trajectories inform the language of emotion. Emotion, 12, 268–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2015). Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1437–1448.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2016). Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 444–455.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kriegeskorte, N., Goebel, R., & Bandettini, P. (2006). Information-based functional brain mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 3863–3868.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(2), 133–143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. D., Weihs, K. L., Herring, A., Hishaw, A., & Smith, R. (2015). Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud's legacy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 55, 594–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (2013). The slippery slope of fear. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 155–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E., & Pine, D. S. (2016). Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: A two-system framework. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 1083–1093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leshinskaya, A., & Caramazza, A. (2016). For a cognitive neuroscience of concepts: Moving beyond the grounding issue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 991–1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Why symbolic processing of affect can disrupt negative affect: Social cognitive and affective neuroscience investigations. In A. Todorov, S. T. Fiske, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Oxford series in social cognition and social neuroscience. Social neuroscience: Toward understanding the underpinnings of the social mind (p. 188–209). Oxford University Press.

  • Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18, 421–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Constructing emotion: The experience of fear as a conceptual act. Psychological Science, 19, 898–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). A functional architecture of the human brain: Emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 533–540.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Russell, J. A. (2006). Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion, 6, 125–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2013). What’s in a word? Language constructs emotion perception. Emotion Review, 5, 66–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Gendron, M., Barrett, L. F., & Dickerson, B. C. (2014). Emotion perception, but not affect perception, is impaired with semantic memory loss. Emotion, 14, 375–387.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Gendron, M., & Satpute, A. B. (2016). Language and emotion. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 579–594). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., MacCormack, J. K., & Shablack, H. (2015). The role of language in emotion: Predictions from psychological constructionism. Frontiers in Psycholology, 6, 444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., & Gendron, M. (2015). Does language do more than communicate emotion? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 99–108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., Wager, T. D., Weber, J., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 1910–1922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan, G., & Clark, A. (2015). Words and the world predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 279–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan, G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). The evocative power of words: Activation of concepts by verbal and nonverbal means. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 170–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCormack, J. K., & Lindquist, K. (2017). Bodily contributions to emotion: Schachter’s legacy for a psychological constructionist view on emotion. Emotion Review, 9, 36–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: Role in paleocerebral functions. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 209–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moors, A., & Scherer, K. R. (2013). The role of appraisal in emotion. In M. D. Robinson, E. Watkins, & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (p. 135–155). The Guilford Press.

  • Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Price, C., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: Relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Annals of Neurology, 47(1), 36–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mur, M., Bandettini, P. A., & Kriegeskorte, N. (2009). Revealing representational content with pattern-information fMRI—An introductory guide. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsn044.

  • Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(3), 184–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nook, E. C., Lindquist, K. A., & Zaki, J. (2015). A new look at emotion perception: Concepts speed and shape facial emotion recognition. Emotion, 15, 569–578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nook, E., Satpute, A. B., & Ochsner, K. (in press). Emotion naming impedes emotion regulation. Affective Science.

  • Nummenmaa, L., & Saarimäki, H. (2017). Emotions as discrete patterns of systemic activity. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 3–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterwijk, S., Lindquist, K. A., Adebayo, M., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). The neural representation of typical and atypical experiences of negative images: Comparing fear, disgust and morbid fascination. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 11–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterwijk, S., Lindquist, K. A., Anderson, E., Dautoff, R., Moriguchi, Y., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). States of mind: Emotions, body feelings, and thoughts share distributed neural networks. Neuroimage, 62, 2110–2128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterwijk, S., Snoek, L., Rotteveel, M., Barrett, L. F., & Scholte, H. S. (2017). Shared states: Using MVPA to test neural overlap between self-focused emotion imagery and other-focused emotion understanding. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1025–1035.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1791–1804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 976–987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., & Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(30), 10127–10134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2018). Understanding emotion with brain networks. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 19–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773–783.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ralph, M. A. L. (2014). Neurocognitive insights on conceptual knowledge and its breakdown. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, G., Winetraub, Y., Jacob, Y., Kinreich, S., Maron-Katz, A., Shaham, G., et al. (2012). Portraying emotions at their unfolding: a multilayered approach for probing dynamics of neural networks. Neuroimage, 60(2), 1448–1461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, G., Touroutoglou, T., Wilson-Mendenhall, C., Gilam, G. Lin, T., Gonen, T., Jacob, Y., Atzil, S., Admon, R., Bleich-Cohen, M., Maron-Katz, A., Hendler, T. & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Functional connectivity dynamics during film viewing reveal common networks for different emotional experiences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 709–723. *Shared senior authorship.

  • Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., & Braisby, N. (1999). Similarity and categorisation: Neuropsychological evidence for a dissociation in explicit categorisation tasks. Cognition, 71, 1–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, J. B., & Donley, M. P. (2006). Animal studies of amygdala function in fear and uncertainty: Relevance to human research. Biological Psychology, 73, 49–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J.-A., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 329–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Widen, S. C. (2002). A label superiority effect in children's categorization of facial expressions. Social Development, 11, 30–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saarimäki, H., Gotsopoulos, A., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Lampinen, J., Vuilleumier, P., Hari, R., Sams, M., & Nummenmaa, L. (2016). Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2563–2573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Said, C. P., Moore, C. D., Engell, A. D., Todorov, A., & Haxby, J. V. (2010). Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressions in the superior temporal sulcus. Journal of Vision, 10, 11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., Kragel, P. A., Barrett, L. F., Wager, T. D., & Bianciardi, M. (2019). Deconstructing arousal into wakeful, autonomic, and affective varieties. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 19–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., & Lindquist, K. A. (2019). The default mode network's role in emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 851–864.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., Nook, E. C., & Cakar, M. E. (2020). The role of language in the construction of emotion and memory: A predictive coding view. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Neuroscience of enduring change: Implications for psychotherapy (pp. 56–88). New York, NY: Oxford University Press, USA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., Nook, E. C., Narayanan, S., Weber, J., Shu, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2016). Emotions in “black or white” or shades of gray? How we think about emotion shapes our perception and neural representation of emotion. Psychological Science, 27, 1428–1442.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Satpute, A. B., Shu, J., Weber, J., Roy, M., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The functional neural architecture of self-reports of affective experience. Biological Psychiatry, 73, 631–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 719–745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J., & Maer, F. (1975). Right hemisphere lateralization for emotion in the human brain: Interactions with cognition. Science, 190(4211), 286–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shablack, H., & Lindquist, K. A. (in press). The role of language in the development of emotion. In V. LoBue, K. Perez-Edgar, & K. Buss (Eds.), Handbook of emotion development.

  • Shaham, G., & Aviezer, H. (2020). Automatic facial reactions to emotional body expressions are not driven by emotional experience. Emotion.

  • Shinkareva, S. V., Gao, C., & Wedell, D. (2020). Audiovisual representations of valence: A cross-study perspective. Affective Science, 1–10.

  • Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 343–393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for speaking. Proceedings of the thirteenth annual meeting of the berkeley linguistics society, 435-445.

  • Smith, R., & Lane, R. D. (2015). The neural basis of one's own conscious and unconscious emotional states. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2014). Validating the why/how contrast for functional MRI studies of theory of mind. Neuroimage, 99, 301–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Folk explanations of behavior a specialized use of a domain-general mechanism. Psychological Science, 26, 724–736.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P., Ellsworth, E., & Adolphs, R. (2016). The neural basis of understanding the expression of the emotions in man and animals. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 95–105.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Identifying the what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 63–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L., & Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: A case study for colour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 469–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stonnington, C. M., Locke, D. E., Hsu, C.-H., Ritenbaugh, C., & Lane, R. D. (2013). Somatization is associated with deficits in affective theory of mind. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74(6), 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L., & Goodman, N. D. (2011). How to grow a mind: Statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science, 331(6022), 1279–1285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. (1962). Affect imagery consciousness: Volume I: The positive affects. Springer Publishing Company.

  • Torre, J. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2018). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling as implicit emotion regulation. Emotion Review, 10, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M., & Kousta, S. (2009). Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition, 1(2), 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2864–2885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T. D., Kang, J., Johnson, T., Nichols, T., Satpute, A. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). A Bayesian model of category-specific emotional brain responses. PLoS Computational Biology, 11(4), e1004066.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 219–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in " my" insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40(3), 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1105–1127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition, 85(3), 223–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review, 114(2), 245–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yik, M. S., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 600–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, C. B., Raz, G., Everaerd, D., Beckmann, C. F., Tendolkar, I., Hendler, T., Fernández, G., & Hermans, E. J. (2017). Dynamic shifts in large-scale brain network balance as a function of arousal. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2), 281–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yuvaraj, R., Murugappan, M., Norlinah, M. I., Sundaraj, K., & Khairiyah, M. (2013). Review of emotion recognition in stroke patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 36(3–4), 179–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Department of Graduate Education of the National Science Foundation (1835309), by the Division of Brain and Cognitive Sciences of the National Science Foundation (1947972), and by the Division of Brain and Cognitive Sciences of the National Science Foundation (1551688).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ajay B. Satpute.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Handling Editor: Wendy Berry Mendes

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Satpute, A.B., Lindquist, K.A. At the Neural Intersection Between Language and Emotion. Affec Sci 2, 207–220 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-021-00032-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-021-00032-2

Keywords

Navigation