Skip to main content
Log in

The Relationship Between Vocabulary and Word Reading Among Head Start Spanish–English Bilingual Children

  • Published:
Early Childhood Education Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study, we investigated the concurrent and longitudinal relationships between vocabulary and word reading across Spanish and English. One hundred and seventeen 4- to 5-year-old Spanish–English bilingual children attending Head Start programs in the United States were tested for their Spanish and English word reading twice, 5 months apart. We also tested the children’s Spanish and English vocabulary and phonological awareness at Time 1. We used multiple regression models to examine the predictive value of vocabulary to word reading cross-linguistically and longitudinally. Results showed that within (Spanish or English) language and concurrent predictions were stronger than cross-language and longitudinal predictions; however, Spanish vocabulary was a significant and unique predictor of English word reading longitudinally. Spanish phonological awareness also played an important role in the relationship between vocabulary and word reading. Our results suggest that helping Spanish-speaking children build their Spanish vocabulary can also improve their English word reading ability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Cirino, P. T., Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2009). Pictures and words: Spanish and English vocabulary in classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 897–911. doi:10.1037/a0015817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlo, M., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C., Dressler, C., Lipman, D., et al. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215. doi:10.1598/RRQ.39.2.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Glaeser, B., & Ferko, D. (2007). Speech perception, vocabulary, and the development of reading skills in English among Korean- and English-speaking children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 154–166. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2009). From grapheme to word in reading acquisition in Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 583–601. doi:10.1017/s0142716409990038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, L. Q., Wu, S., & Daraghmeh, A. (2012). Profiles in bilingualism: Factors influencing kindergartners’ language proficiency. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40, 25–34. doi:10.1007/s10643-011-0491-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoğlu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodrich, J. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Farver, J. M. (2013). Do early literacy skills in children’s first language promote development of skills in their second language? An experimental evaluation of transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 414–426. doi:10.1037/a0031780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Collins, P., Baciu, I., & Gebotys, R. (2008). Predictors of Grade 2 word reading and vocabulary learning from Grade 1 variables in Spanish-speaking children: Similarities and differences. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23, 11–24. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00259.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., & Mueller, J. (2009). Are first- and second-language factors related in predicting second-language reading comprehension? A study of Spanish-speaking children acquiring English as a second language from first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 330–344. doi:10.1037/a0014320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, C., Lawrence, F., & Miccio, A. (2007). Bilingual children’s language abilities and early reading outcomes in Head Start and kindergarten. Language, Speech & Hearing Services In Schools, 38, 237–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.111.3.662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvard University and Center for Applied Linguistics. (2002a). Phonological awareness test. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvard University and Center for Applied Linguistics. (2002b). Habilidad Fonológica. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jalongo, M. R., & Sobolak, M. J. (2011). Supporting young children’s vocabulary growth: The challenges, the benefits, and evidence-based strategies. Early Childhood Education Journal, 38(6), 421–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kena, G., Musu-Gillette, L., Robinson, J., Wang, X., Rathbun, A., Zhang, J., Wilkinson-Flicker, S., Barmer, A., & Dunlop Velez, E. (2015). The condition of education 2015 (NCES 2015-144). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch.

  • Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Smith, K. E., Assel, M. A., & Gunnewig, S. B. (2006). Enhancing early literacy skills for preschool children: Bringing a professional development model to scale. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 306–324. doi:10.1037/a0013842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Crosson, A. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Pierce, M. (2010). Uneven profiles: Language minority learners’ word reading, vocabulary, and reading comprehension skills. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 475–483. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2010.09.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2011). The gap between Spanish speakers’ word reading and word knowledge: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 82, 1544–1560. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01633.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352. doi:10.1037/a0026744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muñoz-Sandoval, A. G., Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2005). Batería III Woodcock-Muñoz: Pruebas de aprovechamiento. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2010). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 189–208. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 235–257. doi:10.1080/10888430701344306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E., & Cheung, A. (2005). A synthesis of research on language of reading instruction for English language learners. Review of Educational Research, 75, 247–284. doi:10.3102/00346543075002247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, B. (2006). Foundations of behavioral statistics: An insight-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 5–20. doi:10.1023/A:1021789804977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warne, R. T. (2011). Beyond multiple regression: Using commonality analysis to better understand R2 results. Gifted Child Quarterly, 55, 313–318. doi:10.1177/0016986211422217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007). The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 1093–1109. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/076).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (13CYY027) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (13wkpy37).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jing Zhao.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhao, J., Dixon, L.Q., Quiroz, B. et al. The Relationship Between Vocabulary and Word Reading Among Head Start Spanish–English Bilingual Children. Early Childhood Educ J 45, 27–34 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-015-0764-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-015-0764-8

Keywords

Navigation