Skip to main content
Log in

Do explicit number names accelerate pre-kindergarteners’ numeracy and place value acquisition?

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this longitudinal study is to investigate whether an early childhood intervention using an explicit and transparent number naming system will have a lasting benefit to English and Spanish speaking children in their mathematics achievement related to number sense by accelerating their acquisition of concepts of numeracy and place value recognition. Statistical analysis of the results of the Stanford Achievement Test administered to school children in grades 1 through 5 at a school in Texas (USA) over a 6-year period of time showed consistent differences between those children who had participated in the intervention program by learning to count using explicit numbers names in Pre-Kindergarten and those who had not participated in the program. The advantages of this study were most apparent in children in the fourth grade, 6 years after their Pre-Kindergarten experience with explicit number names, particularly with those who had 2 years of experience with explicit number names.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, R., & Wu, M. (Eds.). (2003). Programme for international student assessment (PISA): PISA 2000 technical report. Paris: OECD Publishing. doi:10.1787/9789264199521-en

    Google Scholar 

  • Alsawaie, O. N. (2004). Language influence on children’s cognitive number representation. School Science and Mathematics, 104(3), 105–111. doi:10.1111/j.1949-8594.2004.tb17990.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauford, J. E. (2005). Connections between names of numbers and student performance in mathematics. In D. F. Berlin & A. L. White (Eds.), Collaboration for the global improvement of science and mathematics education. Columbus: The International Consortium for Research in Science and Mathematics Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauford, J. E., & Browning, S. T. (2007). The effects of the Use of explicit number names on mathematical understanding and performance mathematics education in a global community. Charlotte: Ninth International Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bright Education. (2015). Stanford Achievement Test (9th Ed.) (SAT-9) [website]. Retrieved from http://brighted.funeducation.com/Practice/Stanford-SAT-9

  • Browning, S. (2008). Language and number values: The influence of the explicitness of number names on childrens’ understanding of place value (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses http://search.proquest.com/docview/304835322?accountid=7139

  • Browning, S. T., & Beauford, J. E. (2011). Language and number values: The influence of the explicitness of number names on children’s understanding of place value. Investigations in Mathematical Learning, 4(2), 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, R. A., & Threlfall, J. (2004). One, two, three and counting. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 55(1), 3–26. doi:10.1023/b:educ.0000017676.79430.dc

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bugden, S., & Ansari, D. (2011). Individual differences in children’s mathematical competence are related to the intentional but not automatic processing of Arabic numerals. Cognition, 118(1), 32–44. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(2), 299–315. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z. Y., Cowell, P., Varley, R., & Wang, Y. C. (2009). A cross-language study of verbal and visuospatial working memory span. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(4), 385–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, Z., & Chan, L. K. S. (2005). Chinese number naming advantages? Analyses of Chinese pre-schoolers’ computational strategies and errors. International Journal of Early Years Education, 13(2), 179–192. doi:10.1080/09669760500171279

  • Cheng, L., Li, L., Kirby, J., Qiang, H., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2010). English language immersion and students’ academic achievement in English, Chinese and mathematics. Evaluation & Research in Education, 23(3), 151–169.

  • Cotter, J. (2000). Using language and visualization to teach place value. Teaching Children Mathematics, 7(2), 108–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Smedt, B., Noel, M. P., Gilmore, C., & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(2), 48–55. doi:10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001

  • Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 218–224. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowker, A., Bala, S., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Linguistic influences on mathematical development: How important is the transparency of the counting system? Philosophical Psychology, 21(4), 523–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C., Grandau, L., & Sugiyama, P. A. (2001). Achievable numerical understanding for all young children. Teaching Children Mathematics, 7(9), 522–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., Choi, Y. E., Lopez, L. S., Netley, R., & Chao-Yuan, C. (1997). Happy birthday to you: Early mathematical thinking of Asian, South American, and U.S. children. In. T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An international perspective (pp. 163–207). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press

  • Habegger, L. (2010). Number concept and rhythmic response in early childhood. Music Education Research, 12(3), 269–280. doi:10.1080/14613808.2010.504810

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrera, A., & Macizo, P. (2010). The production of symbolic and non-symbolic numerals. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 926–934. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, I. D., Price, G. R., & Ansari, D. (2010). Common and segregated neural pathways for the processing of symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 49, 1006–1017. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.071

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imbo, I., & LeFevre, J. (2009). Cultural differences in complex addition: Efficient Chinese versus adaptive Belgians and Canadians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1465–1476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. A., Thornton, C. A., Putt, I. J., Hill, K. M., Mogill, A. T., Rich, B. S., et al. (1996). Multidigit number sense: A framework for instruction and assessment. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 310–336. doi:10.2307/749367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K., Major, S., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2000). Ordinal knowledge: Number names and number concepts in Chinese and English. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 129–139.

  • Miura, I. T. (1987). Mathematics achievement as a function of language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(1), 79–82. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.79.1.79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T. (2001). The influence of language on mathematical representations. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics: 2001 yearbook of the national council of teachers of mathematics (pp. 53–62). Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Zuber, J., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H. C. (2011). Early place-value understanding as a precursor for later arithmetic performance – A longitudinal study on numerical development. Research in Developmental Studies, 32, 1837–1851. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.012

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, J. E., & Franks, D. J. (2010). Why do some children have difficulty learning mathematics? Looking at language for answers. Preventing School Failure, 54(2), 111–118. doi:10.1080/10459880903217861

  • Mullis, I. V., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., O’Connor, K. M., Chrostowski, S. J., Gregory, K. D., … & Smith, T. A. (2001). Mathematics benchmarking report: TIMSS 1999—Eighth grade. Chestnut Hill, MA: International Study Center. Retrieved from http://timssandpirls.bc.edu/timss1999b/mathbench_report/t99b_math_report.html

  • Ng, S., & Rao, N. (2010). Chinese number words, culture, and mathematics learning. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 180–206. doi:10.3102/0034654310364764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noren, E. (2015). Agency and positioning in a multilingual mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 89(2), 167–184. doi:10.1007/s10649-015-9603-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson Education. (2015). Stanford Achievement Test Series (10th Ed.) [website]. Retrieved from http://www.pearsonassessments.com/learningassessments/products/100000415/stanford-achievement-test-series-tenth-edition.html

  • Pixner, S., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., Nuerk, H. C., & Kaufmann, L. (2011). Whorf reloaded: Language effects on nonverbal number processing in first grade – A trilingual study. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 108, 371–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sousa, D. (2008). How the brain learns mathematics. Thousand Oaks: Corwin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Texas Education Agency. (2005). Professional development appraisal system: Teacher manual. Austin: Texas. Retrieved from http://www4.esc13.net/uploads/pdas/docs/PDAS_Teacher_Manual.pdf

  • Texas Education Agency. (2008). Texas prekindergarten guidelines. Retrieved from http://tea.texas.gov/index2.aspx?id=2147495508

  • Tsamir, P., Tirosh, D., Tabach, M., & Levenson, E. (2010). Multiple solution methods and multiple outcomes - Is it a task for kindergarten children? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 73(3), 217–231. doi:10.1007/s10649-009-9215-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Luit, J., & Van der Molen, M. (2011). The effectiveness of Korean number naming on insight into numbers in Dutch students with mild intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(2011), 1941–194. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.028

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Oers, B. (2001). Educational forms of initiation in mathematical culture. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 46, 59–85. doi:10.1023/a:1014031507535

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Lin, E., Tanase, M., & Sas, M. (2008). Revisiting the influence of numerical language characteristics on mathematical achievement: Comparison among China, Romania, and U.S. International Electronic Journal of Mathematics Education, 3(1), 24–46.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was partially funded by the University of the Incarnate Word School of Graduate Studies and Research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suzanne D. Magargee.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Magargee, S.D., Beauford, J.E. Do explicit number names accelerate pre-kindergarteners’ numeracy and place value acquisition?. Educ Stud Math 92, 179–192 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-016-9682-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-016-9682-y

Keywords

Navigation