Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Longitudinal Trajectories of Sustained Attention Development in Children and Adolescents with ADHD

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study characterizes changes in sustained attention ability over ages 9–14, and whether longitudinal trajectories of attention development differ between persistent ADHD, remitted ADHD and control groups. The Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) was administered to 120 children with ADHD and 123 controls on three occasions between ages 9 and 14. Trajectories of sustained attention development, indicated by changes in SART performance (standard deviation of response time [SDRT], omission errors, and ex-Gaussian parameters sigma and tau), were examined using generalized additive mixed models. For all measures there was a significant main effect of age; response time variability and number of omission errors improved linearly as children aged. However, children with ADHD had significantly greater SDRT, tau and omission errors than controls across waves. There were no significant group differences in sigma, indicating that the greater overall response time variability (SDRT) observed in ADHD was likely driven by more intermittent long responses (larger tau). Trajectories of sustained attention performance did not differ between children with persistent ADHD or ADHD in remission. Longitudinal trajectories of sustained attention development are comparable between ADHD and controls, however children with ADHD (regardless of remission status) display a performance deficit equivalent to typical controls 1–3 years younger. Findings highlight the need for continued clinical support for children in remission from ADHD and provide support for tau as an endophenotype of ADHD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding authors, PT and TS, upon reasonable request.

References

  • Agha, S. S., Zammit, S., Thapar, A., & Langley, K. (2013). Are parental ADHD problems associated with a more severe clinical presentation and greater family adversity in children with ADHD? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 22(6), 369–377.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agnew-Blais, J. C., Polanczyk, G. V., Danese, A., Wertz, J., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2016). Evaluation of the persistence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(7), 713–720.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Agnew-Blais, J. C., Polanczyk, G. V., Danese, A., Wertz, J., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2019). Are changes in ADHD course reflected in differences in IQ and executive functioning from childhood to young adulthood? Psychological Medicine, 1–10.

  • Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723.

    Google Scholar 

  • Australian Bureau of Statistics (2013). Census of population and housing: Socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA), Australia, 2011. cat. no. 2033.0. 55.001.

  • Avisar, A., & Shalev, L. (2011). Sustained attention and behavioral characteristics associated with ADHD in adults. Applied Neuropsychology, 18(2), 107–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bédard, A. C. V., Stein, M. A., Halperin, J. M., Krone, B., Rajwan, E., & Newcorn, J. H. (2015). Differential impact of methylphenidate and atomoxetine on sustained attention in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(1), 40–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, I., Slobodin, O., Aboud, M., Melamed, J., & Cassuto, H. (2013). Maturational delay in ADHD: Evidence from CPT. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 691.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Betts, J., Mckay, J., Maruff, P., & Anderson, V. (2006). The development of sustained attention in children: The effect of age and task load. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3), 205–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). The effects of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on employment and household income. Medscape General Medicine, 8(3), 12.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Seidman, L. J., Petty, C. R., Fried, R., Doyle, A. E., Cohen, D. R., Kenealy, D. C., & Faraone, S. V. (2008). Effects of stimulant medication on neuropsychological functioning in young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(7), 1150–1156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Ball, S. W., Fried, R., Doyle, A. E., Cohen, D., Henderson, C., & Faraone, S. V. (2009). Are cognitive deficits in ADHD related to the course of the disorder? A prospective controlled follow-up study of grown up boys with persistent and remitting course. Psychiatry Research, 170(2–3), 177–182.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Evans, M., Small, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). How persistent is ADHD? A controlled 10-year follow-up study of boys with ADHD. Psychiatry Research, 177(3), 299–304.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Clarke, A., Lomedico, A., & Faraone, S. V. (2011). Predictors of persistent ADHD: An 11-year follow-up study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(2), 150–155.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brocki, K. C., Tillman, C. M., & Bohlin, G. (2010). CPT performance, motor activity, and continuous relations to ADHD symptom domains: A developmental study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(2), 178–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brod, M., Schmitt, E., Goodwin, M., Hodgkins, P., & Niebler, G. (2012). ADHD burden of illness in older adults: A life course perspective. Quality of Life Research, 21(5), 795–799.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, R. C. (2005). Sustained attention in patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Clinical Rehabilitation, 19(2), 188–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, C. H., Rijsdijk, F., McLoughlin, G., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., et al. (2016). Cognitive and neurophysiological markers of ADHD persistence and remission. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(6), 548–555.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, D. R., Seth, S., Pedroso, S., Usala, T., Currie, J., & Gagliano, A. (2014). Effects of methylphenidate on cognitive functions in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a systematic review and a meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 76(8), 603–615.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3rd edition: Manual: Multi-Health Systems.

  • Conners, C. K., Staff, M, Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & Barnes, J. (2000). Conners’ continuous performance test II (CPT II v. 5). Multi-Health Syst Inc, 29, 175–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deault, L. C. (2010). A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41(2), 168–192.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 15(3), 331–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di Trani, M., Casini, M. P., Capuzzo, F., Gentile, S., Bianco, G., Menghini, D., et al. (2011). Executive and intellectual functions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with and without comorbidity. Brain and Development, 33(6), 462–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Roche, R. A., Hogan, M. J., Reilly, R. B., & Robertson, I. H. (2004). Behavioural and physiological impairments of sustained attention after traumatic brain injury. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 403–414.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doehnert, M., Brandeis, D., Imhof, K., Drechsler, R., & Steinhausen, H.-C. (2010). Mapping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from childhood to adolescence—No neurophysiologic evidence for a developmental lag of attention but some for inhibition. Biological Psychiatry, 67(7), 608–616.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Döpfner, M., Hautmann, C., Görtz-Dorten, A., Klasen, F., Ravens-Sieberer, U., & Group BS. (2015). Long-term course of ADHD symptoms from childhood to early adulthood in a community sample. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(6), 665–673.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fortenbaugh, F. C., DeGutis, J., Germine, L., Wilmer, J. B., Grosso, M., Russo, K., & Esterman, M. (2015). Sustained attention across the life span in a sample of 10,000: Dissociating ability and strategy. Psychological Science, 26(9), 1497–1510.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development. Nature Neuroscience, 21(3), 315–323.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, T. P., & Marder, K. (2017). Statistical approaches to longitudinal data analysis in neurodegenerative diseases: huntington’s disease as a model. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17(2), 14.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gmehlin, D., Fuermaier, A. B., Walther, S., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Lange, K. W., et al. (2016). Attentional lapses of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in tasks of sustained attention. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(4), 343–357.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greven, S., & Kneib, T. (2010). On the behaviour of marginal and conditional AIC in linear mixed models. Biometrika, 97(4), 773–789.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haack, L. M., Jiang, Y., Delucchi, K., Kaiser, N., McBurnett, K., Hinshaw, S., & Pfiffner, L. (2017). Parental cognitive errors mediate parental psychopathology and ratings of child inattention. Family Process, 56(3), 716–733.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686–702.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halperin, J. M., Trampush, J. W., Miller, C. J., Marks, D. J., & Newcorn, J. H. (2008). Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: Profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(9), 958–966.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harpin, V. A. (2005). The effect of ADHD on the life of an individual, their family, and community from preschool to adult life. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 90(suppl 1), i2–i7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Henríquez-Henríquez, M. P., Billeke, P., Henríquez, H., Zamorano, F. J., Rothhammer, F., & Aboitiz, F. (2015). Intra-individual response variability assessed by ex-Gaussian analysis may be a new endophenotype for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 197.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., Curry, J. F., Tonev, S., Eugene Arnold, L., Keith Conners, C., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., & Hechtman, L. (2006). Reaction time distribution analysis of neuropsychological performance in an ADHD sample. Child Neuropsychology, 12(2), 125–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., Fan, C., Jassy, J. S., & Owens, E. B. (2007). Neuropsychological functioning of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder followed prospectively into adolescence: Evidence for continuing deficits? Neuropsychology, 21(2), 263–273.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, C. W., Scott, S., & Rubia, K. (2011). Investigation of cool and hot executive function in ODD/CD independently of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(10), 1035–1043.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. A., Kelly, S. P., Bellgrove, M. A., Barry, E., Cox, M., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2007a). Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 630–638.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Dáibhis, A., Watchorn, A., Keavey, M., Fitzgerald, M., Gallagher, L., Gill, M., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2007b). Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2234–2245.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. A., Barry, E., Bellgrove, M. A., Cox, M., Kelly, S. P., Dáibhis, A., Daly, M., Keavey, M., Watchorn, A., Fitzgerald, M., McNicholas, F., Kirley, A., Robertson, I. H., & Gill, M. (2008). Dissociation in response to methylphenidate on response variability in a group of medication naive children with ADHD. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1532–1541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karalunas, S. L., Geurts, H. M., Konrad, K., Bender, S., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Annual research review: Reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders: Measurement and mechanisms of a proposed trans-diagnostic phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(6), 685–710.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klarborg, B., Skak Madsen, K., Vestergaard, M., Skimminge, A., Jernigan, T. L., & Baaré, W. F. (2013). Sustained attention is associated with right superior longitudinal fasciculus and superior parietal white matter microstructure in children. Human Brain Mapping, 34(12), 3216–3232.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3-to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407–428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E. G. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(6), 795–811.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacouture, Y., & Cousineau, D. (2008). How to use MATLAB to fit the ex-Gaussian and other probability functions to a distribution of response times. Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(1), 35–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, G. M., Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J. (2018). A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function performance among children. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leth-Steensen, C., Elbaz, Z. K., & Douglas, V. I. (2000). Mean response times, variability, and skew in the responding of ADHD children: A response time distributional approach. Acta Psychologica, 104(2), 167–190.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, F., Butler, A., & Gilbert, L. (2011). A unified approach to model selection using the likelihood ratio test. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 2(2), 155–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, F. C., Reeve, R. A., Kelly, S. P., & Johnson, K. A. (2017a). Evidence of substantial development of inhibitory control and sustained attention between 6 and 8 years of age on an unpredictable go/no-go task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 157, 66–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, F. C., Reeve, R. A., Kelly, S. P., & Johnson, K. A. (2017b). Sustained attention to a predictable, unengaging go/no-go task shows ongoing development between 6 and 11 years. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(6), 1726–1741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, H. Y., Hwang-Gu, S. L., & Gau, S. F. (2015). Intra-individual reaction time variability based on ex-Gaussian distribution as a potential endophenotype for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 132(1), 39–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Padmanabhan, A., & O’Hearn, K. (2010). What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 101–113.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K., Rorden, C., Pickard, J., & Robertson, I. H. (2003). Enhancing the sensitivity of a sustained attention task to frontal damage: Convergent clinical and functional imaging evidence. Neurocase, 9(4), 340–349.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAuley, T., Crosbie, J., Charach, A., & Schachar, R. (2014). The persistence of cognitive deficits in remitted and unremitted ADHD: A case for the state-independence of response inhibition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(3), 292–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAvinue, L. P., Vangkilde, S., Johnson, K. A., Habekost, T., Kyllingsbæk, S., Bundesen, C., & Robertson, I. H. (2015). A componential analysis of visual attention in children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(10), 882–894.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Ho, J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Executive functions in girls with ADHD followed prospectively into young adulthood. Neuropsychology, 26(3), 278–287.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Loya, F., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2013). Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: Developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(9), 1005–1015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Østby, Y., Tamnes, C. K., Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Due-Tønnessen, P., & Walhovd, K. B. (2009). Heterogeneity in subcortical brain development: A structural magnetic resonance imaging study of brain maturation from 8 to 30 years. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(38), 11772–11782.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rebok, G. W., Smith, C. B., Pascualvaca, D. M., Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., & Kellam, S. G. (1997). Developmental changes in attentional performance in urban children from eight to thirteen years. Child Neuropsychology, 3(1), 28–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I. H., & O’Connell, R. (2010). Vigilant attention. Attention and time, 79, 88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rucklidge, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in ADHD: Implications for psychosocial treatments. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 8(4), 643–655.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rucklidge, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics, 33(2), 357–373.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, A. E., Ford, T., & Russell, G. (2015). Socioeconomic associations with ADHD: Findings from a mediation analysis. PLoS One, 10(6), e0128248.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Salomone, S., Fleming, G. R., Bramham, J., O’Connell, R. G., & Robertson, I. H. (2016). Neuropsychological deficits in adult ADHD: Evidence for differential attentional impairments, deficient executive functions, and high self-reported functional impairments. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1087054715623045.

  • Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6(2), 461–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sciberras, E., Efron, D., Schilpzand, E. J., Anderson, V., Jongeling, B., Hazell, P., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Nicholson, J. M. (2013). The Children’s attention project: A community-based longitudinal study of children with ADHD and non-ADHD controls. BMC Psychiatry, 13(1), 18.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 28–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., et al. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(49), 19649–19654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Sharp, W., Evans, A., & Greenstein, D. (2012). Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 72(3), 191–197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shemmassian, S. K., & Lee, S. S. (2012). Comparing four methods of integrating parent and teacher symptom ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34(1), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, T. J., Genc, S., Anderson, V., Efron, D., Hazell, P., Nicholson, J. M., Kean, M., Malpas, C. B., & Sciberras, E. (2016). Developmental brain trajectories in children with ADHD and controls: A longitudinal neuroimaging study. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 59.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skounti, M., Philalithis, A., & Galanakis, E. (2007). Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide. European Journal of Pediatrics, 166(2), 117–123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Failures of sustained attention in life, lab, and brain: Ecological validity of the SART. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2564–2570.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sobeh, J., & Spijkers, W. (2012). Development of attention functions in 5-to 11-year-old Arab children as measured by the German test battery of attention performance (KITAP): A pilot study from Syria. Child Neuropsychology, 18(2), 144–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P., & Shalev, L. (2013). The role of sustained attention and display medium in reading comprehension among adolescents with ADHD and without it. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(1), 431–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamm, L., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., O’Brien, K. M., Hawk, L. W., & Epstein, J. N. (2012). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 500–508.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tamm, L., Epstein, J. N., Peugh, J. L., Nakonezny, P. A., & Hughes, C. W. (2013). Preliminary data suggesting the efficacy of attention training for school-aged children with ADHD. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 16–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamnes, C. K., Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Østby, Y., & Walhovd, K. B. (2012). Becoming consistent: Developmental reductions in intraindividual variability in reaction time are related to white matter integrity. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(3), 972–982.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamnes, C. K., Herting, M. M., Goddings, A.-L., Meuwese, R., Blakemore, S.-J., Dahl, R. E., Güroğlu, B., Raznahan, A., Sowell, E. R., Crone, E. A., & Mills, K. L. (2017). Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: A multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(12), 3402–3412.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trautmann, M., & Zepf, F. D. (2012). Attentional performance, age and scholastic achievement in healthy children. PLoS One, 7(3), e32279.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tucha, L., Fuermaier, A. B., Koerts, J., Buggenthin, R., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., et al. (2017). Sustained attention in adult ADHD: Time-on-task effects of various measures of attention. Journal of Neural Transmission, 124(1), 39–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Belle, J., van Hulst, B. M., & Durston, S. (2015). Developmental differences in intra-individual variability in children with ADHD and ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12), 1316–1326.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varao-Sousa, T. L., Smilek, D., & Kingstone, A. (2018). In the lab and in the wild: How distraction and mind wandering affect attention and memory. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3(1), 42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, A. J., Epstein, J. N., Rausch, J., Altaye, M., Langberg, J., Newcorn, J. H., Hinshaw, S. P., Hechtman, L., Arnold, L. E., Swanson, J. M., & Wigal, T. (2011). Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test and ADHD symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(6), 853–864.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vrieze, S. I. (2012). Model selection and psychological theory: A discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). Psychological Methods, 17(2), 228–243.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler abbreviated intelligence scale (WASI). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weyandt, L. L., Willis, W. G., Swentosky, A., Wilson, K., Janusis, G. M., Chung, H. J., et al. (2014). A review of the use of executive function tasks in externalizing and internalizing disorders. In Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 69-87): Springer.

  • Wickens, T. D. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfinger, R. (1993). Covariance structure selection in general mixed models. Communications in Statistics: Simulation and Computation, 22(4), 1079–1106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolraich, M. L., Hagan, J. F., Allan, C., Chan, E., Davison, D., Earls, M., et al. (2019). Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 144(4), e20192528.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N. (2004). Stable and efficient multiple smoothing parameter estimation for generalized additive models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 99(467), 673–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N. (2011). Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Statistical Methodology), 73(1), 3–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N. (2017). Generalized additive models: an introduction with R (2nd ed.): CRC/Taylor & Francis.

  • Yan, C., Zhou, H., Wei, W., Wang, Y.-J., Cui, L., Chan, R. C., et al. (2018). Developmental trajectories of attention in typically developing Chinese children: A four-wave longitudinal study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 43(6), 479–496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all of the many families and schools for their participation in the Neuroimaging of the Children’s Attention Project.

Funding

The study was funded by the National Medical Health and Research Council of Australia (NHMRC; project grant #1065895). The Children’s Attention Project was as funded by an NHMRC project grant #1008522 and a grant from the Collier Foundation. This research was conducted at the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute and Deakin University. It was supported by the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, The Royal Children’s Hospital, The Royal Children’s Hospital Foundation, Department of Paediatrics at The University of Melbourne and the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program. ES is supported by an NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (#1110688) and a veski Inspiring Women’s Fellowship. DE is supported by a Clinical Scientist Fellowship from the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Phoebe Thomson or Timothy J. Silk.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Philip Hazell or his employer has received payment from Eli Lilly and Janssen for consultancies; Eli Lilly, Janssen, Novartis and Shire for participation in advisory boards; Eli Lilly, Janssen, Pfizer and Shire for speaker’s bureau; Eli Lilly and Celltech for the conduct of clinical trials. All other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Approval

This study was approved by the Royal Children’s Hospital Melbourne Human Research Ethics Committee and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards.

Consent to Participate

Written informed consent was obtained from the parents.

Consent for Publication

Not applicable.

Code Availability

All code implemented in the current study were based on detailed R documentation for Generalized Additive Mixed Models from https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/mgcv/versions/1.8-31/topics/gamm. Custom code that yielded the findings of the current article are available from the corresponding authors upon request.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 8547 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thomson, P., Vijayakumar, N., Johnson, K.A. et al. Longitudinal Trajectories of Sustained Attention Development in Children and Adolescents with ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 48, 1529–1542 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00698-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00698-5

Keywords

Navigation