1 Introduction

Academic entrepreneurship, defined as the commercial application of academic research, is an increasingly important aspect of academic life. In most institutions, academic researchers are strongly encouraged (or even required) to disclose their inventions to the university, and to actively participate in the commercialisation of their research findings (Owen-Smith and Powell 2001; Agrawal and Henderson 2002; Murray 2002; Siegel et al. 2003). However, several recent studies have highlighted the existence of a gender gap in academic entrepreneurship. Female academics are less likely to disclose their inventions (Thursby and Thursby 2005), hold a patent (Whittington and Smith-Doerr 2005; Ding et al. 2006) or create a new enterprise based on their research (Rosa and Dawson 2006). This is surprising given that the gender gap in other measures of academic productivity, such as publishing, is closing (Xie and Shauman 2003).

A number of possible explanations have been identified, including supply-side factors such as lack of industry experience, greater reliance of institutional support, lower levels of seniority and choice of research area by female academics, and demand-side factors such as lower visibility, exclusion from networks and gender discounting (Murray and Graham 2007; Stephan and El-Ganainy 2007). The relevance of some of these factors, particularly those relating to networks and lack of experience, have been confirmed by female academics as part of qualitative case-study interviews (Murray and Graham 2007). Moreover, there is significant quantitative evidence to show that personal and institutional characteristics, such as level of seniority, academic field and degree of institutional support, have an effect on academic entrepreneurship at all stages of the process (Stephan et al. 2007; Haeussler and Colyvas 2011). In particular, Colyvas et al. (2012) find that once a range of personal and institutional characteristics are accounted for, there is no further gender gap in the likelihood of disclosure of inventions, or in the likelihood that disclosures are converted into licenses.

Running through the discussion in the literature are two issues of significance. First, there is now substantial evidence to suggest that rates of academic entrepreneurship are greater in disciplines and fields which have a lower representation of female academics (Rosa and Dawson 2006). Similarly, academic entrepreneurship is greater among senior academics with wide-ranging networks and experience of commercialisation, and an overwhelmingly large proportion of those academics are male (Stephan and El-Ganainy 2007). This raises the (as yet unresolved) question of whether female academics with lower propensities to engage in entrepreneurship are self-selecting into disciplines and career paths that further exclude them from entrepreneurial opportunities.

Second, there is limited evidence on the entrepreneurial intentions and outcomes for academics in disciplines other than the engineering, medicine and the physical sciences. An understanding of the commercialisation activities of academics in the arts, social sciences and humanities is key to explaining the choices of female academics, and their resulting career outcomes. For instance, academics in the creative arts are widely engaged in both consulting and the creation of enterprises based on academic research (Abreu and Grinevich 2014), but it is unclear how this affects the overall gender gap in academic entrepreneurship.

This study aims to address both of these gaps in the literature by analysing the entrepreneurial activities of male and female academics based at a wide range of academic institutions and from a broad range of disciplines. We focus particularly on understanding why the choices of male and female academics differ, and use a non-parametric matching procedure to analyse whether the entrepreneurial outcomes are different for male and female academics who are otherwise very similar in terms of their personal and institutional characteristics. We use the creation of enterprises based on university research (or “spinouts”) as our measure of academic entrepreneurship, as this is the most comparable measure across academic disciplines, and provides a clear parallel to measures of entrepreneurship in the wider literature.

The remainder of the paper is organised as follows. Section 2 provides an overview of the literature on the gender gap in academic entrepreneurship, and the possible explanations that have been advanced in the literature. Section 3 describes our data and methods. Section 4 provides descriptive evidence on the gender gap and the different explanations identified in the literature. Section 5 presents the evidence on the issue of self-selection. Section 6 concludes and discusses policy implications.

2 Is there a gender gap in academic entrepreneurship?

A large literature has explored the existence of a gender gap in scientific careers, both within and outside academia. Women are less likely than men to study science at university, less likely to hold academic positions in the sciences, and significantly less likely to hold senior faculty positions (Xie and Shauman 2003). Of the possible explanations for these disparities, the most widely-held theory is the so-called “leaky pipeline” model, which argues that women drop out of the scientific career path at various points in their life, such as when choosing which subjects to study in school, transitioning to higher education, continuing on to graduate school, applying for faculty positions, and achieving tenure. For instance, in a wide-ranging study of the gender gap in science, Xie and Shauman (2003) find that most of the early-stage gender gaps (such as the proportion of girls choosing science subjects at high school) have closed over time, but that self-selection by women into specific career paths that are seen as more compatible with family life has persisted. This includes opting to specialise in fields that are seen as less competitive and more amenable to flexible working, and for which there are fewer geographical constraints (particularly for women with school-age children). These choices are also reflected in measures of academic productivity, such as publishing. For instance, a number of studies have found that female academics publish less often than their male counterparts, although there is evidence to suggest that this gap has decreased over time (Cole and Zuckerman 1984; Long 1992; Thursby and Thursby 2005), and that while female academics publish fewer articles, their publications have greater impact, as measured using citations (Long 1992).

The academic gender gap is also evident in a variety of measures of research commercialisation, from early-stage intentions, to patenting, licensing and the creation of spinouts. Focusing on the early (or “ex-ante”) stage of the process, Goel et al. (2015) use data from a survey of German scientists working at Max Planck institutes, and find that female academics have significantly lower propensities towards entrepreneurship, defined as a lower perceived attractiveness of creating a spinout. Similarly, using data on invention disclosures, Colyvas et al. (2012) find that US male faculty members are slightly more likely to report an invention (37 vs. 32% for women), and significantly more likely to report multiple inventions to their institution’s Technology Transfer Office (TTO) than their female colleagues. This corroborates earlier evidence by Thursby and Thursby (2005), which shows that despite few differences in publications by gender, the probability of disclosure of an invention by a male academic is 43% higher than that of an individual academic.

Similar results have been found in the context of patenting, with significantly fewer female academics holding patents, relative to their male counterparts. For instance, using data US-based academic life scientists, Ding et al. (2006) find that 6% of women in the sample hold patents, versus 13% of the men. The likelihood that an individual academic has not patented up to a given year of their tenure is higher for women at all career stages, and the gender gap increases over time (Ding et al. 2006). Interesting, the academic context appears to exacerbate this gender gap: Whittington and Smith-Doerr (2005) show that US male scientists in industry patent 1.4 times as much as female scientists, but the gender gap is 2.3 times in academia. As with publishing, there is no difference in the quality of the patents (as measured by patent citations) of male and female scientists (Whittington and Smith-Doerr 2005). Similar findings have been reported for other measures of academic entrepreneurship, such as spinouts. Although based on a relatively small sample, Rosa and Dawson (2006) find that only 12% of the spinouts in their UK sample were founded by women. Female academics also tend to engage in fewer types of commercialisation activity (Haeussler and Colyvas 2011).

3 Disentangling the explanations

While the presence of a significant gender gap in academic entrepreneurship is well documented, there is still considerable controversy over the precise reasons for this gap, which in turn has implications for the appropriate policy responses. A number of studies have investigated whether the gender gap remains once individual and institutional factors are accounted for. This would imply that, although a gender gap in outcomes exists, we are able to explain it with reference to constraints, and resulting constrained choices, affecting female academics. For instance, Colyvas et al. (2012) find that gender has no significant effect on the likelihood of disclosing inventions once individual characteristics (such as academic rank, publishing activity, and external research funding) have been accounted for. Similarly, the authors find no effect of gender on the likelihood that a disclosure becomes a license (although there is a significant gender gap in the volume of disclosures). The authors conclude that the observed gender gap is due to “occupational and resource factors, reflecting the volume of engagement, rather than discrete proclivity to commercialize or the level of success in doing so” (Colyvas et al. 2012, p. 486).

However, an unresolved issue that remains is the extent to which female academics are responding to these constraints by modifying their behaviour, and whether their actions would be different if their circumstances changed. This warrants a closer look at the potential factors that could affect their engagement with entrepreneurship. Several studies have discussed these factors in detail, and it is helpful to classify them into supply- and demand-side explanations, following Stephan and El-Ganainy (2007) and Murray and Graham (2007). The supply-side explanations are those relating to the decision by the academic to engage in entrepreneurial activities, and the resources available to do this (skills, networks, interest), while the demand-side explanations relate to discrimination and lack of opportunities.

On the supply side, one important factor that affects entrepreneurship is the research area or academic field. As is well documented in the literature, some research areas are more conducive to commercialisation than others. For instance, research in the life sciences lends itself readily to commercial exploitation since fundamental research and applied work tend to co-evolve (Murray 2002; Stephan et al. 2007). The same is true for other areas in science which primarily involve use-inspired basic research, that is, basic research that is also inspired by considerations of use (Stokes 1997). Conventional measures of academic entrepreneurship such as disclosures, patents, licences and spinouts are often lower in the arts and humanities, partly because copyright and trademarks are more common forms of intellectual property protection in these disciplines, and partly because external engagement in the arts and humanities is generally based on more informal types of activities such as giving public lectures and organising exhibitions (Abreu and Grinevich 2013, 2014). If women are over-represented in these disciplines (either through choice or due to discrimination), and/or under-represented in areas of the sciences that are more conducive to commercialisation, then this might help to explain the gender gap in the more traditional measures of academic entrepreneurship.Footnote 1

A second supply-side explanation, which is also partly a demand-side explanation (in terms of the issues of visibility that it raises), is the notion that women tend to occupy less senior positions in academia, and may therefore choose to focus on advancing their academic careers, rather than on engaging in entrepreneurial activities. This is particularly true if women are constrained by family obligations that place severe pressures on their time (Ding et al. 2006; Rosa and Dawson 2006).Footnote 2 Family circumstances, such as marital status, career breaks to look after children or elderly parents, and tied moves concerning the job of their partner, can have a significant effect on female entrepreneurship (Rosa and Dawson 2006). A related issue is the importance of previous commercial or business experience in encouraging future entrepreneurial behaviour. Female academics tend to have lower levels of exposure to industry and business, and therefore rely more heavily on the TTO (Stephan and El-Ganainy 2007). A number of studies have found support for the latter hypothesis, for instance, in case study interviews of female life-scientist, Ding et al. (2006) and Murray and Graham (2007) find that women are more likely to rely on the TTO for industry contacts, advice and encouragement. Similarly, Rosa and Dawson (2006) find that women are more likely to cite the shortcomings of the TTO as an obstacle to commercialisation. In a set of regressions to explain the propensity towards entrepreneurship, Goel et al. (2015) find that previous industry experience and “TTO needed for commercialisation” had larger predicted effects for female than for male academics. Being based at a larger and more prestigious university could mitigate some of the negative impacts of a lack of visibility to external partners, since larger, research-intensive institutions tend to have better established and more experienced TTOs (Siegel et al. 2007).

A final set of supply-side explanations relate to psychological factors. There is evidence to suggest that women tend to be more risk-averse than men in the context of business or financial ventures (Eckel and Grossman 2008; Croson and Gneezy 2009; Borghans et al. 2009). This may lead them to avoid potentially riskier entrepreneurial activities such as spinouts, and instead opt for less formal methods of commercialisation (such as consultancy work), or to avoid entrepreneurial activities altogether. Related to this is the finding that women tend to dislike competitive behaviour, and therefore avoid activities perceived to involve competition with their peers (Murray and Graham 2007). The literature has also found that women are less likely to actively seek to “sell” their research, and numerous studies document that female academics believe they are left out of entrepreneurial ventures due to “not being asked” (Rosa and Dawson 2006; Murray and Graham 2007). In addition, there is evidence that women feel more ambivalent towards the ethics of the commercialisation process (Murray and Graham 2007), although this factor may be changing as more PhD students and junior faculty are exposed to commercial links from the outset of their careers (Ding et al. 2006).

On the demand side, the explanations revolve around the level of interest of external partners in the entrepreneurial ventures of female academics. Given that fewer female academics have previous commercial experience (as discussed above), we might expect that their networks include fewer contacts within industry or business, which might make it more difficult to “sell” a patent or business idea. Female academics also tend to have fewer PhD students, and are therefore less likely to become involved in entrepreneurial ventures through requests from former students (Murray 2004). The lack of wide-ranging networks been found in qualitative research to be one of the biggest constraints to female entrepreneurship (Murray 2004; Ding et al. 2006). In addition, women may face difficulties in raising finance for their ventures, partly because their lower academic rank makes them less visible to potential partners (Rosa and Dawson 2006), and partly due to discrimination, since venture capitalists tend to be male, and may have a “higher comfort level with men than with women and thus contribute to the gender gap” (Stephan and El-Ganainy 2007).

As discussed above, a key unresolved question in the literature is the extent to which these findings are affected by selection bias. More precisely, whether the gender gap would close if female academics had the same attributes and institutional contexts as their male counterparts. In other words, the issue is not simply that female academics have a lower rate of engagement in absolute terms (the constant in the regression). It is that their endowments (the values of the explanatory variables) are different, and the effects of those endowments on academic entrepreneurship (the coefficients) are also different. In fact, there are usually very few (or even no) female academics with precisely the same attributes as their male counterparts, such as working in a top research university, in a highly-competitive scientific sub-field, holding a senior position, with previous experience of entrepreneurship. While existing studies have accounted for differences in the attributes by including a wide range of explanatory variables (to account for differences in the endowments), or by running separate regressions for male and female academics (to account for differences in both the endowments and their effects), there are very few studies that have attempted to address the issue of selection bias. A notable exception, although not in a quantitative context, is Murray and Graham (2007), who interviewed all female academics in the life sciences working in “Big School”, a prestigious university, and a matched sample of male academics for comparison purposes. Crucially, the male interviewees were identified by asking the female interviewees to provide details of “one or more of their male peers within Big School” (Murray and Graham 2007). Although the resulting sample is not random, it has the advantage of ensuring (in as much as possible) that the male and female samples are comparable in terms of the attributes. There are no similar approaches based on large-scale, quantitative studies, and our paper aims to fill this gap.

4 Data and methods

4.1 Data sources and limitations

Our unit of analysis is the individual academic, and our data originates from a survey conducted in 2008–2009 as part of a wider project on the knowledge-exchange activities of UK academics. The survey was administered using an online web-survey tool, and was sent to all academics in the UK who were publicly listed on their institution’s website in 2008, resulting in a sampling frame of 126,120 academics from across all UK higher education institutions and disciplines.Footnote 3 The achieved sample was 22,556, for an overall response rate of 18%. The survey included questions on a wide range of entrepreneurial activities (including informal activities), personal characteristics, the nature of the respondent’s teaching and research, questions about the culture and ethics of academic entrepreneurship, and the geographical scope of the respondent’s entrepreneurial activities. The questions in the survey refer to the 3-year period prior to the survey (i.e., to 2005–2008).

Due to the method used in constructing the sampling frame, which involved collecting names and contact details from university websites, it is possible that the survey under-sampled junior academics (in particular, research assistants and teaching assistants), since they are less likely to be listed on department websites, given their relatively short contracts. It is also possible that senior academics were more likely to respond to the survey (given their greater experience in entrepreneurial activities), although we are unable to verify this since the sampling frame does not contain personal details such as academic position.Footnote 4 Our achieved sample includes a higher proportion of professors, readers and senior lecturers (the most senior positions), and a lower proportion of lecturers and research associates or assistants (the most junior positions), relative to the population (see Table 13 in Appendix for details). In addition, the male/female gender ratio among the professors in our sample is higher than it is in the population, while there is no difference in the gender ratio for other, more junior, positions (Table 14 in Appendix). It is likely that academics with little involvement in entrepreneurial activities were less likely to complete the survey, despite our best efforts, including several statements, both on the survey and on the project website, urging them to do so. Since female academics are less likely to be involved in entrepreneurial activities, it may be that female academics not involved in entrepreneurial activities were less likely to complete the survey and that our sample therefore includes a higher than expected number of female entrepreneurs. Our findings showing the existence of a significant gender gap are therefore more likely to understate the scale of the problem, than to overstate it.

Similarly, the survey slightly oversampled academics in the disciplines more likely to be involved in entrepreneurial activities (such as the health sciences, engineering and the physical sciences, and business and media), and undersampled academics in disciplines with low levels of engagement (such as the social sciences, humanities and education). Again this is probably due to lower response rates by academics who are not involved in entrepreneurial activities (Table 15 in Appendix). The male/female gender ratio is also higher in the sample than in the population in these disciplines, suggesting that we may be underestimating the gender gap in the most entrepreneurial research fields (Table 16 in Appendix).

In order to ensure a broadly comparable sample, we exclude from the analysis academics who reported that they were not involved in either teaching or research activities (these are mainly senior university administrators and those on secondment to external organisations), resulting in a sample of 22,300 individuals, based at 150 UK higher education institutions.Footnote 5 We distinguish between the 24 research-intensive universities who form part of the Russell Group (referred to as “top research university” in the analysis), and the remaining 126 institutions.

4.2 Methods

Our analysis consists of four stages. First, we use a set of descriptive statistics to give a first indication as to whether the gender patterns of academic entrepreneurship are affected by the factors identified in the literature (and discussed in Sect. 2). We focus in particular on the type of entrepreneurial activity undertaken (patenting, licensing, spinouts and consultancy work), and consider their correlation with personal characteristics such as type of research, academic discipline, level of seniority, ethical views on entrepreneurship, previous experience, and the level of institutional support. We also provide preliminary evidence to show that a selection bias mechanism might be at work. The descriptive analysis also shows the results of a test of equality of proportions for each gender difference considered.

Table 1 Gender gap by type of activity (% involved in each activity)
Table 2 Informal and non-commercial activities (% of all respondents)
Table 3 Type of research (% who describe their research as…)
Table 4 Spinout activity by discipline (% of faculty that is female,  % involved in spinouts)
Table 5 Seniority (% male/female faculty in each rank;  % involved in spinouts)
Table 6 Personal and institutional constraints to academic entrepreneurship (% of all respondents)
Table 7 Who makes the initial approach? (% of those who have been involved in academic entrepreneurship, more than one may apply)
Table 8 Changing role of commercial activities (% who agree with the statement “over the past few years, universities have gone too far in attempting to meet the needs of industry to the detriment of their core teaching and research roles”)
Table 9 Scope for commercialisation of research (% who agree with the statement “my research…”)

Second, we estimate two probit models to quantify the extent of the gender gap after controlling for a range of explanatory variables. Our outcome of interest for this part of the analysis is spinout activity, or more precisely, whether the individual has been involved in the creation of an enterprise based on his/her research in the past 3 years.Footnote 6 The first model includes only a female dummy variable, the coefficient of which is equivalent to the gender gap measured as the difference between the rate of male and female spinout activity. The second model includes a female dummy variable, and a number of explanatory variables identified in the literature as contributing to the academic entrepreneurship gender gap. The second model is equivalent to the approach used in Colyvas et al. (2012), who find that once all relevant factors are included in the regression, there is no longer a statistically significant gender gap in academic entrepreneurship (see the discussion in Sect. 3 for more details).

Table 10 Probit models for spinout activity, with female dummy variable

Third, we use a Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition approach to disaggregate the differences in academic entrepreneurship between male and female academics into a part that is explained by differences in the endowments, and a second part that is explained by differences in the coefficients (Blinder 1973; Oaxaca 1973). Our outcome variable for this part of the analysis is also spinout activity. The Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition starts from the premise that there are two groups, Group 1 (the reference group, with the highest outcome), and Group 2. In our case, Group 1 is comprised of male academics, and Group 2 of female academics. We use the “two-fold” Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition, which allows us to separate the mean difference between the two groups D into two components:

$$ D = E + C $$
(1)

where E is the part that is explained by differences in the endowments (explanatory variables), and C is the part that is explained by differences in the coefficients. Intuitively, E captures the expected change in the mean outcome of female academics, if female academics had the attributes of the male academics in the sample, whereas C shows the expected change in the mean outcome of female academics, if female academics had the coefficients of the male academics in the sample. We are thus able to analyse whether it is the systematic differences in the attributes of female academics that explain the gender gap, or whether what matters is how those attributes translate into entrepreneurial outcomes (or both).

Table 11 Oaxaca decomposition for spinout activity by gender

Finally, we extend the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition to control for the possibility that there may be combinations of attributes for which it is possible to find male academics in the sample, but not female academics. Failing to control for these differences (henceforth the problem of “differences in the supports”), which may be due to choices made throughout academic careers, leads to misspecification, and in the context of the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition, tends to overestimate the component of the gap that is due to differences in the coefficients. We control for this misspecification problem using a non-parametric technique developed by Ñopo (2008). Intuitively, the gap is now disaggregated into four components:

$$ D = M + X + F + D_{0} $$
(2)

where D is the gender gap as before; M is the part explained by differences between male academics who have characteristics that cannot be matched to female characteristics, and those who have characteristics that can be matched; X is the part explained by differences in the distribution of male and female characteristics over the matched sample (corresponds to E in the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition, under the assumption of no selection bias); F is the part explained by differences between females who have characteristics that can be matched to male characteristics, and those who have characteristics that cannot be matched; and D 0 is the unexplained part (corresponding to C in the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition). The matching process works as follows (Ñopo 2008, p. 293):

  1. (a)

    Select one female academic from the sample.

  2. (b)

    Select all males that have the same characteristics as the female selected in (a).

  3. (c)

    Using all the individuals selected in (b), construct a synthetic individual whose rate of spinout activity is the average of all of these individuals, and match him to the original female academic.

  4. (d)

    Add the observations for both individuals (the female academic and the synthetic male academic) to their respective samples of matched individuals.

  5. (e)

    Repeat steps (a)-(d) until all of the original sample of female academics has been exhausted.

This non-parametric procedure allows us to establish whether the gender gap is due to differences in the endowments between male and female academics with attributes that can be matched, differences in the coefficients (the unexplained part), or differences due to the fact that male academics have attributes that are highly correlated with academic entrepreneurship, but which are rarely found among female academics (and vice versa). The choice of characteristics for matching is a subjective one; choosing a greater number of characteristics results in a closer match, but restricts the number of male and female academics who fall into the matched sample, and therefore reduces the estimate of X, the proportion of the gap that can be explained by differences in the male and female characteristics over the matched sample. For simplicity, our matching variables are the same variables used as explanatory variables in the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition.

Table 12 Non-parametric Ñopo (2008) decomposition for spinout activity by gender (%)

4.3 Variables included in the analysis

As discussed in Sect. 4.2, the dependent variable used in the decomposition analysis is spinout activity, defined as whether an academic has been involved in the creation of an enterprise based on his or her research. In the descriptive analysis we also consider several other types of entrepreneurial activity, including patenting, licensing and consultancy, all of which are defined as whether an activity of a particular type occurred in the 3 years prior to the survey date. In addition, we consider several more informal activities, including sitting on advisory boards, giving public lectures, engaging in contract research, providing informal advice and participating in exhibitions, all of which are entrepreneurial in nature.

Throughout the analysis, we use a set of explanatory variables that aim to capture the potential explanations for gender bias advanced in the literature, as discussed in Sect. 3. A first set of explanatory variables relate to the personal characteristics of the respondent that have been hypothesised to affect the supply of entrepreneurship, and these include academic position (professor, reader/senior lecturer, lecturer, research fellow, research assistant), type of research based on the Stokes (1997) classification (basic research, user-inspired research, applied research, other), academic discipline (health sciences, biological sciences, engineering and physical sciences, social sciences, business and media, humanities, creative arts, or education), whether the respondent is based at Russell Group research-intensive university (top research university), whether he/she has experience of previously starting or managing a small business (business experience), and whether he/she participates in networks that include potential external partners (network participant).Footnote 7

We also include a number of variables relating to the perceived constraints and ethical/moral views of research commercialisation, identified as part of the psychological explanations for the supply of entrepreneurial activities. These include whether the individual perceives that they are constrained in by a lack of time to fulfill all university roles (lack of time), by difficulty in finding potential external partners (no partners), and by a lack of interest from external organisations (lack of interest). We also include two variables that capture the views of each individual with respect to the ethics and morality of academic entrepreneurship. They indicate whether the individual feels that commercialization has been taken too far (too far), and whether an individual feels that universities are doing too little to meet the needs of business and industry (too little).Footnote 8 A list of the variables included in the analysis, the relevant survey questions, and details of any additional data manipulations used to construct them, can be found in Table 17 in Appendix.Footnote 9

Our analysis is limited to the supply side explanations, since we only capture the determinants and constraints reported by the individual academics, and do not have access to information on their potential or actual external partners. However, a few of the factors, such as the level of seniority, and participation in networks that also feature non-academic partners, can shed some light on the demand side constraints faced by female academics. Likewise, the perception of a lack of interest from external partners may reflect both supply- and demand-side explanations. We discuss these issues in more detail in the next section.

5 Quantifying the gender gap

We start the analysis by quantifying the gender gap in academic entrepreneurship, using descriptive statistics. As shown in Table 1, the gender gap among UK academics is fairly large and statistically significant, particularly for patenting (6.1%) and consultancy work (6.8%). It is also fairly significant for licensing (3.9%) and spinouts (3.2%). These numbers are generally in keeping with those found in earlier studies. A gender gap is also apparent for more informal measures of entrepreneurship, such as sitting on advisory boards (7.3%), contract research (5.6%), giving public lectures (6.3%), and providing informal advice to non-academic partners (5.3%). In keeping with our discussion in Sect. 2, it is likely that less formal activities that are not usually channelled through the TTO favour male academics, who tend to have wider networks and tend to be more visible to external organisations due to their greater seniority levels and previous involvement in commercial ventures. The only measure without a statistically significant gender gap is participating in public exhibitions (0.9%), which tends to be the preserve of academics in the creative arts (Table 2).

We next explore the evidence relating to the theories discussed in Sect. 2. One of the most salient findings in the literature is the relationship between the type of research undertaken and the gender gap. Some academic disciplines and sub-fields, such as the life sciences, lend themselves readily to commercialisation, since the research is both fundamental, but also inspired by considerations of use. If female academics are less likely to be involved in this type of research, and more likely to concentrate in disciplines with little tradition of entrepreneurship, then this would go some way towards explaining the entrepreneurship gender gap. Table 3 illustrates this issue, by showing the percentage of male and female academics who describe their research as (a) basic, (b) use-inspired basic, (c) applied, or (d) other.Footnote 10 These categories are based on Stokes (1997), who argues that basic and use-inspired research are conducive to the most lucrative and wide-ranging commercial ventures, while applied research is often too narrowly defined to lead to substantial commercial activity. As shown in Table 3, female academics are under-represented in both basic and use-inspired basic research, and over-represented in applied research, suggesting that they are under-represented in the research activities that the literature has found are more likely to result in successful commercialisation, although this result may not necessarily hold for all academic disciplines.Footnote 11

Similarly, Table 4 shows that female academics are more likely to work in disciplines with low overall rates of spinout activity, such as the social sciences, humanities and education (the health sciences are an exception), and less likely to work in engineering, the physical sciences and biological sciences, which have high rates of spinout activity. Interestingly, the gender gap tends to be greater in disciplines with higher spinout activity, such as the health sciences, engineering and the physical sciences, business and media, and the creative arts. This suggests that women are both under-represented in high-entrepreneurship disciplines, and are less likely to be involved in academic entrepreneurship even if they are in the “right” disciplines. It is also in keeping with research showing that the majority of business start-ups by women are in sectors with high rates of female employment, and that women are less likely to start a new business in non-traditional sectors (Rosa and Dawson 2006).

We also find evidence for several of the theories discussed in Sect. 2 which highlight the constraints faced by female academics. As shown in Table 5, women are significantly less likely to be full professors, and are over-represented in all other (lower rank) academic positions. We would expect this to lead to lower rates of spinout activity, as professors are the most visible faculty members in academia, with the widest networks and greatest recognition among potential industrial and business partners. However, we also find that, even if female academics have the rank of professor, they are less likely than their male counterparts to be involved in entrepreneurial activities (2.8% of female professors are involved in spinouts, compared to 7.8% of male professors). This is partly due to the other constraints faced by female academics (such as time pressures and lack of business experience), and partly due to the uneven research field and subject mix, with a larger proportion of female professors being in academic disciplines such as the humanities and education which have lower overall rates of entrepreneurial activity.

Female academics are, given their lower ranks and lower levels of previous experience,Footnote 12 more likely to rely on institutional support provided by the TTO. As illustrated in Table 6, while male academics are more likely to be disparaging of the services provided by the TTO (partly due to greater exposure to the TTO), female academics are likely to regard the resources provided as insufficient, suggesting they have the greater need for them. Similarly, and as discussed in the literature, female academics are more likely to “wait to be asked”. Table 7 shows that female academics are more likely to say that commercial ventures resulted from mutual actions following a formal meeting, while male academics are more likely to mention own actions (which is equivalent to “asking”), or actions involving the TTO.

Finally, we explore two of the psychological explanations proposed by the literature. Table 8 shows the percentage of male and female survey respondents who agree with the statement “over the past few years, universities have gone too far in attempting to meet the needs of industry to the detriment of their core teaching and research roles”. Our aim was to test whether female academics are more ambivalent (or, alternatively, more negative) about research commercialisation, as highlighted by Murray and Graham (2007). Our findings support the ambivalence view, with male academics feeling more strongly (either for or against) on this issue, while female academics are significantly more likely to select “neither agree nor disagree”. We do not, however, find evidence that female academics have more negative views on commercialisation.

Finally, we find that female academics are more likely to state that their research is of relevance to non-commercial organisations (as shown on Table 9), while male academics are more likely to state that it has been applied in a commercial setting, or that it is in an area of interest to industry/business. This may be because female academics are overrepresented in academic disciplines that are strongly associated with the public and not-for-profit sectors (such as the health sciences, social sciences, and humanities), and also partly because they feel more ambivalent about research commercialisation (as shown in Table 8), and are therefore more comfortable engaging with external organisations on a not-for-profit basis.

As a final step, we test whether the gender gap persists after controlling for a range of personal characteristics associated with academic entrepreneurship, as identified by the literature. The variables included are those discussed in Sect. 4.3, and correspond to the factors explored in the descriptive analysis above. We estimate two probit models for spinout activity, one with only a female dummy variable as explanatory variable, and one that also includes the other determinants listed in Sect. 4.3. The results are shown in Table 10. As expected, the probit model with only the female dummy variable captures the difference in spinout activity between male and female academics, and is equivalent to the gender gap shown in Table 1 (3.2 percentage points). The second column shows the model with all of the explanatory variables included, in addition to the female dummy variable. The first result to note is that the coefficient of the female dummy variable is now much reduced, and is down to 0.8 percentage points. While this is not quite as dramatic a result as that found by Colyvas et al. (2012), it does indicate that a large proportion of the gender gap can be explained by differences in the explanatory variables. The remainder of the gender gap is then likely to be due to discrimination and other unknown factors, as we explore in the next section. The results for the other explanatory are also as expected. Being a professor raises the probability of spinout activity by 1.1 percentage points, relative to being a lecturer. Being involved in use-inspired research or applied research is associated with higher probabilities of spinout activity (0.7 and 0.8 percentage points, respectively), relative to being involved in basic research. The academic discipline also has an important effect on the outcome variables; academics in engineering and the physical sciences are more likely to be involved in spinouts (by 0.8 percentage points), while academics in the humanities, social sciences, and education are less likely to be (by 1.2, 1.2 and 1.0 percentage points). Being based at a top research university is associated with higher levels of engagement (0.4 percentage points), while having entrepreneurial experience has a substantial and positive effect (12.4 percentage points). Participating in networks with external partners is associated with a small increase in spinout activity (1.1 percentage points), ethical considerations only have a small effect, and the existence of perceived constraints has no statistically significant effect. These results are in keeping with the findings of the descriptive analysis, and indicate that although differences in the explanatory variables explain some of the gender gap, there is a component that remains unexplained.

6 Disaggregating the gender gap

As discussed above, the attributes of female and male academics tend to differ in several key ways. Female academics are more likely to work in academic disciplines for which there is less of a tradition of academic entrepreneurship, to hold more junior positions, to have smaller networks and therefore rely more on the TTO for their entrepreneurial activities, and to feel more ambivalent about the commercialisation of their research. Since these characteristics are also associated with lower rates of academic entrepreneurship, a useful question to ask is whether the observed differences in academic entrepreneurship between male and female academics are due to differences in the attributes, or to differences in the way that those attributes translate into academic entrepreneurship. We investigate this question for our main variable of interest, spinout activity, using the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition method described in Sect. 4.2.

Table 11 shows the results of the decomposition, based on a model that includes the variables discussed in Sect. 4.3. As shown in the first line of Table 11, the overall gender gap in spinout activity is 3.3 percentage points, and this gap is statistically significant. The Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition shows that 61% of this gap can be explained by differences in the endowments, meaning that if female academics had the average attributes of the male academics in the sample, the gender gap would close by this amount. The part of the gender gap that can be explained by differences in the coefficients (the behavioural responses to those attributes), is 39%. This can be interpreted as the degree to which the gap would close if the female academics in the sample had the coefficients of the male academics. This is often referred to as the “unexplained component”, since it may partly indicate gender-based discrimination. We can disaggregate the gender gap further by looking at the decomposition for individual variables, as shown in the remainder of Table 11. The endowments that explain the greatest proportions of the gender gap are whether the individual is a professor (14%), academic discipline, particularly engineering and the physical sciences (14%), and whether the individual has previous experience in starting or running a small business (23%). This implies that if female academics were as likely to be professors as the average male academic in the sample, the gap would close by 14%, and similarly for the other two variables. Of particular interest is the large effect associated with previous experience, as it implies that less exposure to commercial activities leads to greater barriers to engagement in the future, resulting in a significant constraint to female academic entrepreneurship.

Few of the individual differences in the coefficients are statistically significant. One exception is the type of research, where the gender gap would increase if female academics responded to applied research in the same way as male academics do (−25%), since working in applied research is more likely to lead to spinout activity for female academics.Footnote 13 Another exception is academic discipline, where our results show that female academics in the health sciences would be more likely to engage in commercialisation if they had the same response as male academics do to being in that discipline (15.7%). In other words, male academics in the health sciences are much more likely than female academics in the same discipline to generate spinouts, probably because female academics in the health sciences are more likely to be in sub-fields where there are fewer opportunities for research commercialisation. This is after controlling for academic position, experience and other variables.

As discussed in Sect. 4.2, however, the results of the Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition are likely to be biased if there are few (or no) females in the sample with the same attributes as those of the males in the sample. In order to address this, we use the Ñopo decomposition approach outlined in Sect. 4.2, and the results are shown in Table 12. In order to interpret the results, we express the gender gap as a percentage of the female spinout rate. The overall gender gap is 207%. This can be decomposed into a component that is due to differences in the individual attributes for individuals that can be matched to the other group (31%), a component that is due to differences in the coefficients, and is therefore “unexplained” (76%), a component explained by differences between males who have attributes that cannot be matched to those of the females in the sample, and males with attributes that can be matched (114%), and a component explained by differences between females with attributes that can be matched to the males in the sample, and attributes that cannot be matched (−14%). Our results therefore indicate that the majority of the gender gap can be explained by characteristics of male academics (seniority, type of research, discipline, experience) that are highly conducive to successful commercialisation, but which are very rare among female academics. After this, the second most important component is differences in the behavioural responses of female academics, or the “unexplained component”. Our results therefore indicate the existence of significant differences in the characteristics of female and male academics, which in turn translate into substantially different rates of academic entrepreneurship.

7 Conclusions

Our study analyses the determinants of the gender gap in academic entrepreneurship among UK-based academics from across a wide range of academic disciplines. We focus on spinout activity as a measure of academic entrepreneurship, as it is an easily quantifiable measure, which is appropriate for academics working in the sciences, social sciences, humanities and the arts. We explore the relevance of the different explanations that have been identified in the literature, and show that the average female academic in our sample differs from the average male academic in several important ways. Female academics are likely to hold less senior positions, to work in the health sciences, social sciences, humanities and education, to have less prior experience of running a business, and to feel ambivalent about research commercialisation.

All of these characteristics are also correlated with lower rates of academic entrepreneurship, and our Blinder–Oaxaca decomposition analysis shows that these differences in attributes translate into substantial differences in spinout activity. We control for the possibility of selection bias by using the non-parametric Ñopo (2008) matching model, which allows for differences in the distribution of these attributes across male and female academics. Our results indicate that for certain combinations of characteristics that are more prevalent among male academics (being a professor, working on use-inspired basic research, in engineering and the physical sciences, with previous entrepreneurial experience, with positive views on the ethics of commercialisation) there are few or no female academics with the same mix of characteristics. This in turn explains a substantial part of the entrepreneurship gender gap.

In the current policy context, our results have several important implications. First, we find that while female academics face significant obstacles to commercialising their research, this does not necessarily imply that the gender gap will close once these obstacles are removed. Female academics differ from their male counterparts in several important ways, at least some of which may be due to conscious choices, such as self-selection into research areas that are more conducive to links with the public and not-for-profit sectors. Rather than impose a one-size-fits-all policy, TTOs should adapt to support ventures that differ from the traditional science-based commercial venture.

Second, female academics are severely constrained in their opportunities for commercialisation by their less senior status, and lack of entrepreneurial experience. There is scope for universities to support female academics by improving their career progression opportunities, in a way that is flexible and compatible with family life. Shadowing schemes and networking events would help to close the gender gap in this context.

Finally, we have shown that the gender gap in academic entrepreneurship exists across the entire range of academic disciplines, and is not solely confined to the sciences. Greater visibility of the entrepreneurial activities in areas other than engineering and the life sciences would help to highlight the specific obstacles faced by female academics working in these fields. More research is also needed to understand the specific constraints faced by female academics in disciplines other than the sciences and engineering.