Skip to main content
Log in

Theory of Mind in Patients with Epilepsy: a Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

  • Review
  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ability to understand our own thoughts, intentions, beliefs and emotions and those of others (Theory of Mind; ToM) is a high-order social cognitive skill that is vital for social interaction and which has been found to be impaired in patients with epilepsy. Studies examining ToM in patients with epilepsy, however, have yielded inconsistent findings. The main aim of this study is to determine whether the magnitude of ToM deficits varies as a function of the site of epilepsy focus and/or the type of ToM task used. Electronic databases searches included Psychinfo, Medline/PubMed and EMBASE. Studies were included if they examined a group of patients with epilepsy and a group of healthy controls, reported original research, were published in the English language in peer reviewed journals, and used one of five empirically validated measures of ToM: False Belief, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task (RMET), Faux-pas, Strange Stories, Cartoon ToM vignettes. Twelve studies were identified, ten included adults and two included children with epilepsy. Findings revealed marked ToM deficits in adults with focal seizures emanating from core brain regions underpinning ToM: temporal and frontal lobes (frontal lobe epilepsy, FLE; temporal lobe epilepsy, TLE), but not in adults with focal seizures outside the temporal and frontal lobes (extra-TLE/FLE). ToM deficits were also observed in children with generalised seizures (idiopathic generalised epilepsy, IGE). ToM deficits were documented across ToM tasks. In conclusion, ToM deficits represent a robust finding in adults with frontal and temporal epilepsy, but are also found in children with generalised seizures. Further research into ToM is needed, especially in children with epilepsy as early ToM may have cumulative, negative effects on development of social skills that continues into adulthood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amlerova, J., Cavanna, A., Bradac, O., Javurkova, A., Raudenska, J., & Marusic, P. (2014). Emotion recognition and social cognition in temporal lobe epilepsy and the effect of epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy & Behavior, 36, 86–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D., & Frith, C. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, V., Brown, S., Newitt, H., & Hoile, H. (2011). Long-term outcome from childhood traumatic brain injury: intellectual ability, personality, and quality of life. Neuropsychology, 25(2), 176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2009). Intellectual outcome from preschool traumatic brain injury: a 5-year prospective, longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 124(6), e1064–e1071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, V., & Moore, C. (1995). Age at injury as a predictor of outcome following pediatric head injury: a longitudinal perspective. Child Neuropsychology, 1(3), 187–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1995). Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition & Emotion, 9(2–3), 151–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., Pelletier, J., & Homer, B. (2002). Theory of mind and epistemological development: the relation between children’s second-order false-belief understanding and their ability to reason about evidence. New Ideas in Psychology, 20(2), 131–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social sensitivity: detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron‐Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron‐Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high‐functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauminger-Zviely, N. (2013). False-belief task Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorders (pp. 1249–1249): Springer.

  • Black, L., Schefft, B., Howe, S. R., Szaflarski, J. P., Yeh, H. S., & Privitera, M. D. (2010). The effect of seizures on working memory and executive functioning performance. Epilepsy & Behavior, 17(3), 412–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, O., Frith, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’and cognition. Brain, 127(4), 914–928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2 (p. 104). Englewood: Biostat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Broicher, S. D., Kuchukhidze, G., Grunwald, T., Kramer, G., Kurthen, M., & Jokeit, H. (2012). “Tell me how do I feel”–Emotion recognition and theory of mind in symptomatic mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 50(1), 118–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brune, M. (2005). Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind’,and social behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 133(2), 135–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., & Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function. Cognition, 107(2), 663–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, R., Sagun, J., Siddarth, P., Gurbani, S., Koh, S., Gowrinathan, R., & Sankar, R. (2005). Social competence in pediatric epilepsy: insights into underlying mechanisms. Epilepsy & Behavior, 6(2), 218–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallini, E., Lecce, S., Bottiroli, S., Palladino, P., & Pagnin, A. (2013). Beyond false belief: theory of mind in young, young-old, and old-old adults. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 76(3), 181–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Culhane-Shelburne, K., Chapieski, L., Hiscock, M., & Glaze, D. (2002). Executive functions in children with frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(05), 623–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, S. H., & Black, N. (1998). The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 52(6), 377–384.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, C. M., Rickards, H. E., & Cavanna, A. E. (2011). The cognitive impact of antiepileptic drugs. Therapeutic Advances in Neurological Disorders, 4(6), 385–407.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629–634.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, J. (2001). A proposed diagnostic scheme for people with epileptic seizures and with epilepsy: report of the ILAE Task Force on Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia, 42(6), 796–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farrant, A., Morris, R. G., Russell, T., Elwes, R., Akanuma, N., Alarcón, G., & Koutroumanidis, M. (2005). Social cognition in frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 7(3), 506–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 16–15). NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: From brains to culture: Sage.

  • Fournier, N., Calverley, K., Wagner, J., Poock, J., & Crossley, M. (2008). Impaired social cognition 30 years after hemispherectomy for intractable epilepsy: the importance of the right hemisphere in complex social functioning. Epilepsy & Behavior, 12(3), 460–471.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 671–678.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds-a biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11–21.

  • Gelžinienė, G., Jurkevičienė, G., Marmienė, V., Adomaitienė, V., & Endzinienė, M. (2010). Executive functions in adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Medicina (Kaunas, Lithuania), 47(6), 313–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Genizi, J., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Shahar, E., Yaniv, S., & Aharon-Perez, J. (2012). Impaired social behavior in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Journal of Child Neurology, 27(2), 156–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giovagnoli, A. R. (2014). The importance of theory of mind in epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 39, 145–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giovagnoli, A. R., Canafoglia, L., Reati, F., Raviglione, F., & Franceschetti, S. (2009). The neuropsychological pattern of Unverricht–Lundborg disease. Epilepsy Research, 84(2), 217–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giovagnoli, A. R., Franceschetti, S., Reati, F., Parente, A., Maccagnano, C., Villani, F., & Spreafico, R. (2011). Theory of mind in frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy: cognitive and neural aspects. Epilepsia, 52(11), 1995–2002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giovagnoli, A. R., Parente, A., Villani, F., Franceschetti, S., & Spreafico, R. (2013). Theory of mind and epilepsy: what clinical implications? Epilepsia, 54(9), 1639–1646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez-Ibañez, A., Urrestarazu, E., & Viteri, C. (2014). Recognition of facial emotions and identity in patients with mesial temporal lobe and idiopathic generalized epilepsy: an eye-tracking study. Seizure, 23(10), 892–898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J., Cowan, D., Lee, T., & Sachdev, P. (2015). Recent trends in testing social cognition. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(2), 133–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., & Summers, F. (2006). Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: the role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1623–1628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J., & Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Version 5.0.1.: The Cochrane Collaboration.

  • Hrabok, M., Dykeman, J., Sherman, E. M. S., & Wiebe, S. (2013). An evidence-based checklist to assess neuropsychological outcomes of epilepsy surgery: How good is the evidence? Epilepsy & Behavior, 29(3), 443–448.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hynes, C. A., & Mar, R. A. (2009). A case study of long-term cognitive and social functioning following a right temporal lobectomy in infancy. Neurocase, 15(1), 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Y., Hu, Y., Wang, Y., Zhou, N., Zhu, L., & Wang, K. (2014). Empathy and emotion recognition in patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 37, 139–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalbe, E., Schlegel, M., Sack, A. T., Nowak, D. A., Dafotakis, M., Bangard, C., & Kessler, J. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex, 46(6), 769–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaland, N., Møller-Nielsen, A., Smith, L., Mortensen, E. L., Callesen, K., & Gottlieb, D. (2005). The strange stories test. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 14(2), 73–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. T., Kwon, D. H., & Chang, Y. (2011). Impairments of facial emotion recognition and theory of mind in methamphetamine abusers. Psychiatry Research, 186(1), 80–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, H. E. (2006). Social cognition and epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy & Behavior, 8(1), 71–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, P., & Brodie, M. J. (2001). Neuropsychological effects of epilepsy and antiepileptic drugs. The Lancet, 357(9251), 216–222.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ladegaard, N., Larsen, E. R., Videbech, P., & Lysaker, P. H. (2014). Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Research, 216(1), 37–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lew, A. R., Lewis, C., Lunn, J., Tomlin, P., Basu, H., Roach, J., & Martland, T. (2015). Social cognition in children with epilepsy in mainstream education. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 57(1), 53–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., Chiu, M., Yeh, Z., Liou, H., Cheng, T., & Hua, M. (2013). Theory of mind in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(05), 594–600.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martín-Rodríguez, J. F., & León-Carrión, J. (2010). Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: a quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1181–1191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. B., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Young, L., King, D., Paggi, A., Fabri, M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2010). Abnormal moral reasoning in complete and partial callosotomy patients. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 2215–2220.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: meta‐analysis of the relation between language ability and false‐belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mo, S., Su, Y., Chan, R. C., & Liu, J. (2008). Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: the role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Research, 157(1), 21–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151(4), 264–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mutter, B., Alcorn, M. B., & Welsh, M. (2006). Theory of mind and executive function: working-memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(3), 819–835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, M. A., Redoblado, M. A., Lah, S., Sabaz, M., Lawson, J. A., Cunningham, A. M., & Bye, A. M. (2003). Intelligence in childhood epilepsy syndromes. Epilepsy Research, 53(1), 139–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, I. R., McCoy, D., Klobusicky, E., & Ross, L. A. (2012). Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes: a review and theoretical framework. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 8(2), 123–133.

  • Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., & Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain, 130(7), 1718–1731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, S., & Kwon, S. (2008). Cognitive effects of antiepileptic drugs. Journal of Clinical Neurology, 4(3), 99–106.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickup, G. J. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: a systematic review. Psychopathology, 41(4), 206–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(2), 127–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rantanen, K., Eriksson, K., & Nieminen, P. (2012). Social competence in children with epilepsy—a review. Epilepsy & Behavior, 24(3), 295–303.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rajkumar, A. P., Yovan, S., Raveendran, A. L., & Russell, P. S. (2008). Can only intelligent children do mind reading: the relationship between intelligence and theory of mind in 8 to 11 years old. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4(51), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richard-Mornas, A., Mazzietti, A., Koenig, O., Borg, C., Convers, P., & Thomas-Antérion, C. (2014). Emergence of hyper empathy after right amygdalohippocampectomy. Neurocase, 20(6), 666–670.

  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835–1842.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 43(10), 1391–1399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacher, M., Winkler, R., Grunwald, T., Kraemer, G., Kurthen, M., Reed, V., & Jokeit, H. (2006). Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy impairs advanced social cognition. Epilepsia, 47(12), 2141–2146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, P., Lawrence, E., Bramham, J., Brierley, B., Radbourne, C., & David, A. (2007). A prospective study of the effects of anterior temporal lobectomy on emotion recognition and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2783–2790.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, P., Lawrence, E., Radbourne, C., Bramham, J., Polkey, C., & David, A. (2004). The impact of early and late damage to the human amygdala on ‘theory of mind’ reasoning. Brain, 127(7), 1535–1548.

  • Sodian, B., & Kristen, S. (2010). Theory of mind Towards a theory of thinking (pp. 189–201): Springer.

  • Song, H. J., Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2008). Can an agent’s false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition, 109(3), 295–315.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sosa, J. T. R., Ojeda, M. A., & del Rosario, L. R. (2011). Theory of mind, facial recognition and emotional processing in schizophrenia. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition), 4(1), 28–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, V., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., & Young, A. (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 209–220.

  • Sullivan, J. E., & Dlugos, D. J. (2004). Idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Current Treatment Options in Neurology, 6(3), 231–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suurmeijer, T. P., Reuvekamp, M. F., & Aldenkamp, B. P. (2001). Social functioning, psychological functioning, and quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsia, 42(9), 1160–1168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, W. H., Shih, Y. H., Yu, H. Y., Yen, D. J., Lin, Y. Y., Kwan, S. Y., Hua, M. S. (2015). Theory of mind and social functioning in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, ​56(7), 1117–1123.

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child development, 72(3), 655–684.

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suncica Lah.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Details of Quality Rating Items

The following 17 items from the Downs and Black checklist (1998) were employed to assess quality of included studies: 1. Is the hypothesis/aim/objective of the study clearly described? 2. Are the main outcomes to be measured clearly described in the Introduction or Methods section? 3. Are the characteristics of the patients included in the study described clearly? 5. Are the distributions of principal confounders in each group of subjects to be compared described clearly? 6. Are the main findings of the study described clearly? 7. Does the study provide estimates of the random variability in the data for the main outcomes? 9. Have the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up been described? 10. Have actual probability values been reported (for example, 0.035 rather than < 0.05) for the main outcomes except where the probability value is less than 0.001? 11. Were the subjects asked to participate in the study representative of the entire population from which they were recruited? 16. If any of the results of the study were based on ‘data dredging’, was this made clear? 18. Were the statistical tests used to assess the main outcomes appropriate? 20. Were the main outcome measures used accurate (valid and reliable)? 21. Were the patients in different groups recruited from the same population? 22. Were study subjects recruited over the same period of time? 25. Was there adequate adjustment for confounding in the analyses from which the main findings were drawn? 26. Were losses of patients to follow-up taken into account? 27: Did the study have sufficient power to detect a clinically important effect where the probability value for a difference being due to chance is less than 5 %?

Details of Theory of Mind Tasks

Five types of ToM tasks were included in this review. These tasks were selected based on a recent review by Henry et al. (2015), which identified the empirically validated and psychometrically sound behavioural measures of ToM.

False Belief Tasks

Participants are read aloud short stories about a character who has a mistaken belief (i.e., a belief that is inconsistent with their own beliefs and/or or out of line with reality). The participant must correctly infer the mental state of the character by using their cognitive ToM skills. In the simplest version of these tasks, participants must make inferences about the mistaken beliefs of a character with regard to real events (first-order false belief). In more complicated versions of the task, participants must make meta-cognitive inferences: attributing the false belief of one person based on the thoughts of another (second-order false belief). First-order and second-order false belief are commonly grouped together to measure false belief (i.e., ToM) performance. Numerous versions of this task have been developed, which are mostly based on the initial task done by (Wimmer and Perner 1983). This task assesses early cognitive ToM (Henry et al. 2015).

Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET)

Participants are presented with a series of photographs of the eye regions of faces and are asked to select the emotion label that best depicts how a character is feeling. The Reading in the Mind in the Eyes Task (RMET), designed by Baron-Cohen, is the most commonly used of these measures and contains 36 photographs, presented sequentially with four emotion labels for each (Baron‐Cohen et al. 2001). Adaptations of this task have been created for participants from different cultural groups (Jiang et al. 2014). The RMET assesses early affective ToM (Henry et al. 2015).

Faux-Pas Tasks

Participants are read aloud stories that contain a social faux-pas (i.e., scenario in which a character says or does something unintended that may impact upon another characters feelings). Following each story, participants are asked a series of questions that assess their ability to detect the faux-pas (i.e., Did someone say something they shouldn’t have said or something awkward? Who said something they shouldn’t have said or something awkward?); their cognitive ToM (Why do you think he or she said it?); and their affective ToM (How do you think X felt when Y said that?). Each participant obtains a total ToM score by summing responses across questions on the faux-pas stories. They also obtain a score representing their ability to detect the faux pas (Faux-pas hits; calculated by summing responses to the question 1 on each story). Participants are also read control stories that do not contain faux-pas’ and are asked the first comprehension question to assess their ability to rule out non-existent faux-pas and obtain a score representing their ability to rule out non-existant faux-pas (Faux-pas correct rejections; calculated by summing responses to the first question asked on each of the non faux-pas stories). This task assessed advanced ToM (cognitive and affective components) (Henry et al. 2015).

Strange Stories

Participants are read aloud stories involving an interaction between two people, in which characters say or do something that they do not literally mean. The stories contain jokes, metaphors, double bluff, mistakes, persuasion, white lies and/or deception. Participants are asked a series of questions assessing their ability to comprehend the implicit meaning of the story (What it true, what X said?) and then to make inferences about characters’ thoughts, beliefs, intentions and emotional states (Why did X say it?). The original version of this task contains 24 short stories, however shortened versions have been used in recent studies (Happé 1994). This task assesses advanced ToM (cognitive and affective components) (Henry et al. 2015).

Cartoon ToM Tasks

Participants are shown a series of single frame cartoons in which humour depends on what a character mistakenly thought or did not know. Participants are typically asked two questions. The first question is open-ended (implicit form), asking participants why the picture is funny; the second question is presented in a more explicit manner (explicit form), asking what the motives of the character in each picture are. The total score for each type of question ranges from 0 to 20. Some studies have only used the implicit form of the question, however, to receive a correct response, participants must accurately infer what is funny in the story with reference to the thoughts, beliefs, intentions and feelings of characters. This task assesses advanced ToM (cognitive and affective component).

Table 7

Table 7 Studies excluded from meta-analysis

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stewart, E., Catroppa, C. & Lah, S. Theory of Mind in Patients with Epilepsy: a Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 26, 3–24 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-015-9313-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-015-9313-x

Keywords

Navigation