Skip to main content
Log in

The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Can citizens heed factual information, even when such information challenges their partisan and ideological attachments? The “backfire effect,” described by Nyhan and Reifler (Polit Behav 32(2):303–330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2, 2010), says no: rather than simply ignoring factual information, presenting respondents with facts can compound their ignorance. In their study, conservatives presented with factual information about the absence of Weapons of Mass Destruction in Iraq became more convinced that such weapons had been found. The present paper presents results from five experiments in which we enrolled more than 10,100 subjects and tested 52 issues of potential backfire. Across all experiments, we found no corrections capable of triggering backfire, despite testing precisely the kinds of polarized issues where backfire should be expected. Evidence of factual backfire is far more tenuous than prior research suggests. By and large, citizens heed factual information, even when such information challenges their ideological commitments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Google News search for the “backfire”, “backlash”, or “boomerang” effect and the names of Nyhan or Reifler returns over 300 unique articles. The 2010 backfire paper has also enjoyed remarkable academic attention. Among all papers printed in Political Behavior in the last 10 years, “When Corrections Fail” has been cited four times as much as the next most cited paper.

  2. In this way, the apparent difficulty in making one’s policy preferences fit with one’s factual attitudes is redolent of Americans’ struggle to have their policy preferences fit with each other—what Converse (1964) famously described as poor “constraint.”

  3. To avoid the possibility of unintended panel conditioning, we excluded any Turk worker which had been participated in a prior study.

  4. The choice of the OLS model, and the specific measures for agreement, ideology, and correction, were chosen to be consistent with Nyhan and Jason (2010).

  5. This relationship persisted if we compare respondents along the partisan scale. This result is described in Sect. A.14.1 on p. xxvi.

  6. Of course, the attitudinal consequence of this fact remains at a respondent’s discretion, but functional democratic competence would seem to require that voters adopt a common set of basic political facts.

  7. Three articles were taken from study 3: the original Bush WMD article, the piece by Speaker Paul Ryan criticizing President Obama’s policy toward abortion, and Secretary Hillary Clinton claim that twice as many Americans were employed in solar than in the oil industry. Three novel mock articles were also provided: Senator Sanders claiming that the EPA had found fracking was responsible for polluting water supplies, Donald Trump claiming that his tax cut plan would grow federal tax receipts, and Trump claiming that the true unemployment rate was actually higher than 30%. These mock articles can be read in Sect. A.9, which can be found in the appendix on p. xvi. The items can be read in Table 11 on p. xxiii.

  8. For this study, President Obama, Secretary Clinton, and Senator Sanders are deemed liberal speakers, and President Trump and President Bush are deemed conservative speakers.

  9. For instance, the national representative panel who adopted the correction that the flu vaccines did not induce flu infections (Nyhan et al. 2015) or the national representative panel who accepted the correction that the MMR vaccines did not cause autism (Nyhan et al. 2014).

  10. Coppock and McClellan (2017) report an extensive test of the Lucid sample, comparing it to Turk, the Census Bureau’s Current Population Survey, and the American National Election Study’s (ANES) face to face and online samples. Treating the ANES face-to-face sample as the “gold standard”, the Lucid sample is more psychologically similar to the ANES than the Turk sample on the Big-5 personality battery, and better matches the political knowledge and conservatism in the ANES. Coppock and McClellan also test the Lucid sample’s ability to recover treatment effects in canonical social psychology experiments. Both Lucid and Turk samples recover the same framing effect observed the General Social Survey (a massive face-to-face survey instrument), improving the appetite for public spending when it is described as “assistance to the poor” or “caring for the poor” rather than “welfare.” Both Lucid and Turk feature the same framing effect underpinning prospect theory [(the famous Tversky and Kahneman (1983) finding which shows risk tolerance is affected by framing possible outcomes as gains or losses.] Both Lucid and Turk recover indistinguishable experimental effects as observed in Hiscox (2006) in framing attitudes about free trade. Most importantly for this study, the one failed replication was on rumor corrections in the aforementioned Berinsky paper (2017), where Lucid respondents were unusually resistant to corrective information. This suggests that the Lucid sample is at least a comparably demanding sample in which to test factual adherence.

  11. In brief—a weak correction might inadvertently advertise the weakness of the corrective case, or a strong correction might have more obvious factual implications, and therefore inspire more forceful counterargument.

  12. These respondents were recruited on Mechanical Turk.

  13. As a robustness check—there was no significant relationship between ideology and perceived accordance, for any of the tested pairs.

  14. It’s instructive to consider those statement/correction pairs at either end of this spectrum. The statement by Senator Ted Cruz about the incidence of violence targeted at law enforcement, described above, was judged the most proximate correction. At the other end of this continuum is the 2012 claim by Congressman Paul Ryan that “Obama stands for an absolute, unqualified right to abortion—at any time, under any circumstances, and paid for by taxpayers” and the correction that “The number of abortions steadily declined during President Obama’s first term, with fewer abortions in 2012 than any year since 1973.” While Cruz makes a precise claim about the change in the incidence of killings of police officers, Ryan’s statements merely suggested a spike in the incidence of abortion.

  15. An example of a proximate correction is Representative Gutiérrez’s promise that President Obama would be the “champion...[of the] undocumented” paired with the evidence that Obama was a prodigious deporter of these residents. This correction/statement pair was scored 81.7 on a 100-pt scale of accordance.

  16. An example of a distant correction is Governor Romney’s description of the United States using “a credit card ...issued by the Bank of China” and the correction that China holds about 15% of US debt. This correction/statement pair was scored 47.2.

  17. Contra our evidence in “Does Counterargument Explain Our Pattern of Findings?” section.

References

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Leticia Bode, John Brehm, DJ Flynn, Jim Gimpel, Don Green, Will Howell, David Kirby, Michael Neblo, Brendan Nyhan, Gaurav Sood, and the participants at the Center for Stategic Initiatives workshop. Research support was generously furnished by the Cato Institute, and we owe a special debt of gratitude to Emily Ekins and David Kirby. All remaining errors are the responsibility of the authors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Wood.

Additional information

All figures and tables in this paper can be replicated with the syntax available at the Political Behavior dataverse: https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/AGRX5U

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 3926 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wood, T., Porter, E. The Elusive Backfire Effect: Mass Attitudes’ Steadfast Factual Adherence. Polit Behav 41, 135–163 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9443-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9443-y

Keywords

Navigation