Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Children’s oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research investigated the link between oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Study 1 consisted of 61 children (M = 6:1 years) who had experienced 1 year of reading instruction on average. Children’s story retelling was scored for memory and narrative quality. The quality of children’s narratives correlated positively with their reading skill at this age, but narrative quality did not uniquely predict their reading skill 1 year later. Study 2 consisted of 39 children (M = 7:0 years) who had experienced 2 years of reading instruction on average. At this age, the quality of children’s narratives uniquely predicted their reading skill concurrently and 1 year later, even after controlling for their receptive vocabulary and early decoding. These findings have implications for theories of the oral language foundations of reading and for assessment in the early years of reading instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, S. R., Marsh, H. W., Sheppard, M. J., & Sheppard, J. L. (1985). Seven-year longitudinal study of the early prediction of reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 349–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caffrey, E., Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2008). The predictive validity of dynamic assessment: A review. The Journal of Special Education, 41, 254–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Fey, M., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Hamel, J. (1996). Hemi and the Shortie Pyjamas. Auckland: Puffin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devlin, K. (2000). The math gene: How mathematical thinking evolved and how numbers are like gossip. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (2001). Beginning literacy with language. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Poe, M. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 465–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, fourth edition: Manual. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, D. (1974–1975). A six-year study of children who learned to read in school at the age of four. Reading Research Quarterly, 10, 9–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elley, W. B. (1992). How in the world do students read? IEA Study of Reading Literacy. The Hague: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feagans, L., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 308–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24, 123–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hosp, M. K., & Fuchs, L. S. (2005). Using CBM as an indicator of decoding, word reading, and comprehension: Do the relations change with grade? School Psychology Review, 34, 9–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, E. D. Jr. (2003). Reading comprehension requires knowledge—of words and the world. American Educator, Spring, 10–44.

  • Kame’enui, E. J., Fuchs, L., Francis, D. J., Good, R., O’Connor, R. E., Simmons, D. C., et al. (2006). The adequacy of tools for assessing reading competence: A framework and review. Educational Researcher, 35, 3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kame’enui, E., & Simmons, D. (2001). Introduction to this special issue. The DNA of reading fluency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, R. A., Cummings, K. D., Powell-Smith, K. A., & Good, R. H. (2008). Best practices in using dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills for formative assessment and evaluation. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology V (pp. 1181–1204). Bethesda: National Association of School Psychologists.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967/1997). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of Washington Press (Reprinted in Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 3–38).

  • Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and non-word repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministry of Education. (2006). How the decile is calculated. Available: retrieved June 20, 2007 from: http://www.minedu.govt.nz.

  • Nation, K., & Angell, P. (2006). Learning to read and learning to comprehend. London Review of Education, 4, 77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool children’s narratives and performance on the peabody individualized achievement test-revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language, 24, 149–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children’s literacy from mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, E., Haden, C. A., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P. J., Fivush, R., & Ornstein, P. O. (2008). Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model (manuscript under review).

  • Reese, E., Sparks, A., Kalia, V., Long, J., Suggate, S., & Schaughency, E. (2008). Oral narrative skills and reading: Implications for assessment. Paper to be presented in Narrative assessment with preschoolers: Significance and best practices (A. Nicolopoulou, Chair) at the International Association of Child Language, Edinburgh, Scotland.

  • Renfrew, C. (1969). The bus story: A test of continuous speech. North Place, Old Headington: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 235–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F., Speece, D., & Cooper, D. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. Journal of Educational Research, 95, 259–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaughency, E. & Suggate, S. (2007). Evaluation of DIBELS for assessing literacy growth in New Zealand. Paper presented at the 115th Annual Conference of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA.

  • Schaughency, E., & Suggate, S. (2008). Measuring basic early literacy skills amongst year 1 students in New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 43, 85–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 446–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309–1324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S., & Whitehurst, G. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suggate, S. P., Schaughency, E. A., & Reese, E. (2008). Age of beginning formal reading instruction and later literacy: Evidence from longitudinal research. Paper presented at the 28th International Congress of Psychology, Berlin.

  • Tabors, P. O., Snow, C. E., & Dickinson, D. K. (2001). Homes and schools together: Supporting language and literacy development. In D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language (pp. 313–334). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • US Department of Health and Human Services. (2002). Early childhood education and school readiness: Conceptual models, constructs, and measures. Washington, DC: Workshop summary.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from pre-readers to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11–29). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the teachers and pupils at primary schools in Dunedin, New Zealand for their participation. Part of this research was funded with a grant from the Division of Sciences at the University of Otago to the fourth author. Thank you also to Donna Anderson, Amelia Gill, Anna Hamlin, Jillian Hayhurst, Fiona Jack, Amy Meffan, Alana Roughan, and Clark Sim for their help with data collection, transcribing, and coding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elaine Reese.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 6.

Table 6 Examples of narrative quality

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Reese, E., Suggate, S., Long, J. et al. Children’s oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Read Writ 23, 627–644 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9175-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9175-9

Keywords

Navigation