Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Investigating profiles of lexical quality in preschool and their contribution to first grade reading

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This longitudinal study investigated profiles of lexical quality domains in preschool children and the extent to which profile membership predicted reading comprehension in first grade. A latent profile analysis was conducted to classify 420 preschool children on lexical quality domains, including orthography, phonology, morphosyntax, and vocabulary. Regression analysis was used to determine whether profile membership was associated with first grade outcomes across reading comprehension and its components (i.e., listening comprehension and word recognition). Results revealed five profiles of lexical quality which were predictive of all three outcomes in first grade. Children in low lexical quality profiles performed more poorly on the outcome measures than children in the higher lexical quality profiles. Additionally, profile membership did differentially predict later reading outcomes. These results suggest that lexical quality profiles are associated with reading and therefore may offer a means of early identification of children who are susceptible to future reading difficulties.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Given this high correlation, we tested a two-factor confirmatory factor analysis with reading (all measures) and listening comprehension. Model fit was worse than for the three-factor model; thus we retained the three factors.

References

  • Apel, K., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Brimo, D., & Perrin, N. A. (2012). Metalinguistic contributions to reading and spelling in second and third grade students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(6), 1283–1305. doi:10.1007/s11145-011-9317-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Test for reception of grammar (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 278–293. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Nielsen, D. C., Bridges, M. S., & Liu, Y.-S. (2014). Early identification of reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities,. doi:10.1177/0022219414556121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13(2), 195–212. doi:10.1007/BF01246098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C. (2014). Listening text comprehension in preschoolers: A longitudinal study on the role of semantic components. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(5), 793–817. doi:10.1007/s11145-013-9464-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 3–17. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2004). Test of narrative language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(2), 127–160. doi:10.1007/BF00401799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437–447. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.80.4.437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1997). Kaufman brief intelligence test (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Reading comprehension tests vary in the skills they assess: Differential dependence on decoding and oral comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(3), 281–300. doi:10.1080/10888430802132279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 765–778. doi:10.1037/a0015956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2014). Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development, 86(1), 128–144. doi:10.1111/cdev.12293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Apel, K., & Al Otaiba, S. (2013). The relation of linguistic awareness and vocabulary to word reading and spelling for first-grade students participating in response to intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44(4), 337–347. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2013/12-0013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium (2015). Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50(2), 151–169. doi:10.1002/rrq.99.

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium, Farquharson, K., & Murphy, K. A. (2016). Ten steps to conducting a large, multi-site, longitudinal investigation of language and reading in young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00419.

  • Lawson, M. A., & Masyn, K. E. (2015). Analyzing profiles and predictors of students’ social-ecological engagement. AERA Open,. doi:10.1177/2332858415615856.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (2010). Qualitative reading inventory (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, R. J. A., & Pentimonti, M. J. (2016). Introduction to latent class analysis for reading fluency research. In D. K. Cummings & Y. Petscher (Eds.), The fluency construct: Curriculum-based measurement concepts and applications (pp. 309–332). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2007). Test of preschool early literacy. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W. H., MacGinitie, R. K., Maria, K., & Dreyer, L. G. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie reading tests (4th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). MPlus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S. H., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 1031–1039. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02254.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342–356. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00238.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the national early literacy panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535–569. doi:10.1080/10705510701575396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. Precursors of functional literacy, 11, 67–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Rice/wexler test of early grammatical impairment. San Antonia, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, T., Isberner, M.-B., Naumann, J., & Neeb, Y. (2013). Lexical quality and reading comprehension in primary school children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 415–434. doi:10.1080/10888438.2013.764879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74(2), 358–373. doi:10.2307/3696318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(2), 407–418. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4102.407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tambyraja, S. R., Schmitt, M. B., Farquharson, K., & Justice, L. M. (2015). Stability of language and literacy profiles of children with language impairment in the public schools. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(4), 1167–1181. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 407–423. doi:10.1002/acp.1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., Irausquin, R., & Segers, E. (2016). The unique role of lexical accessibility in predicting kindergarten emergent literacy. Reading and Writing, 29(4), 591–608. doi:10.1007/s11145-015-9614-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved +three-step approaches. Political Analysis, 18(4), 450–469. doi:10.1093/pan/mpq025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. M. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals: Preschool (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery tests-revised: Normative update. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service/Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are deeply grateful to the numerous staff, research associates, school administrators, teachers, children, and families who participated. Key personnel at study sites include: Lisa Baldwin-Skinner, Lauren Barnes, Garey Berry, Beau Bevens, Jennifer Bostic, Shara Brinkley, Janet Capps, Beth Chandler, Lori Chleborad, Willa Cree, Dawn Davis, Jaclyn Dynia, Michel Eltschinger, Kelly Farquharson, Tamarine Foreman, Rashaun Geter, Sara Gilliam, Cindy Honnens, Miki Herman, Jaime Kubik, Trudy Kuo, Gustavo Lujan, Junko Maekawa, Carol Mesa, Denise Meyer, Maria Moratto, Kimberly Murphy, Marcie Mutters, Amy Pratt, Trevor Rey, Amber Sherman, Shannon Tierney, Stephanie Williams, and Gloria Yeomans-Maldonado.

Funding

This work was supported by grant # R305F100002 of the Institute of Education Sciences’ Reading for Understanding Initiative.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kimberly A. Murphy.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Please see the “Appendix” section for LARRC project sites and investigator details.

Appendix

Appendix

This paper was prepared by a Task Force of the Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) consisting of Kimberly A. Murphy (Convener), Kate Cain, Hugh Catts, Laura Justice, Richard Lomax, and Jill Pentimonti, along with Kelly Farquharson. LARRC project sites and investigators are as follows:

  • Ohio State University (Columbus, OH): Laura M. Justice (Site PI), Richard Lomax, Ann O’Connell, Jill Pentimonti1, Stephen A. Petrill2, Shayne B. Piasta

  • Arizona State University (Tempe, AZ): Shelley Gray (Site PI), Maria Adelaida Restrepo.

  • Lancaster University (Lancaster, UK): Kate Cain (Site PI).

  • University of Kansas (Lawrence, KS): Hugh Catts3 (Site PI), Mindy Bridges, Diane Nielsen.

  • University of Nebraska-Lincoln (Lincoln, NE): Tiffany Hogan4 (Site PI), Jim Bovaird, J. Ron Nelson.5

  1. 1.

    Jill Pentimonti is now at American Institutes for Research.

  2. 2.

    Stephen A. Petrill was a LARRC co-investigator from 2010-2013.

  3. 3.

    Hugh Catts is now at Florida State University.

  4. 4.

    Tiffany Hogan is now at MGH Institute of Health Professions.

  5. 5.

    J. Ron Nelson was a LARRC co-investigator from 2010-2012.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Murphy, K.A., Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC). & Farquharson, K. Investigating profiles of lexical quality in preschool and their contribution to first grade reading. Read Writ 29, 1745–1770 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9651-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9651-y

Keywords

Navigation