Skip to main content
Log in

To click or not to click: investigating conflict detection and sourcing in a multiple document hypertext environment

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated whether accessing conflicting claims in other documents by means of hyperlinks embedded within currently read documents may facilitate conflict detection and source-content integration. Norwegian undergraduates (n = 85) read multiple conflicting documents on a controversial health-related issue, with half of the conflicting claims across documents hyperlinked and the other half not. Moreover, half of the participants were told that they would get more information by clicking on the links (weak prompting condition) while the other half were additionally told that clicking on the links was necessary to get a more complete understanding of the issue (strong prompting condition). Results indicated that the extent to which participants accessed conflicting claims in other documents via the hyperlinks was positively related to their detection of cross-document conflicts as well as their integration of source-content information. A mediational analysis indicated that conflict detection mediated the effect of accessing conflicting claims via the hyperlinks on source-content integration. No relationship was found between the prompting condition and participants’ selection of the hyperlinks. The theoretical significance as well as the practical value of our findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amichai-Hamburger, Y., Kaynar, O., & Fine, A. (2007). The effects of need for cognition on internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 880–891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2006.03.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andresen, A., Anmarkrud, Ø., & Bråten, I. (2019). Investigating multiple source use among students with and without dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32, 1149–1174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9904-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antonenko, P. D., & Niederhauser, D. S. (2010). The influence of leads on cognitive load and learning in a hypertext environment. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 140–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.10.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barzilai, S., & Eseth-Alkalai, Y. (2015). The role of epistemic perspectives in comprehension of multiple author viewpoints. Learning and Instruction, 36, 86–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barzilai, S., Tzadok, E., & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2015). Sourcing while reading divergent expert accounts: Pathways from views of knowing to written argumentation. Instructional Science, 43, 737–766. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-015-9359-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Björnsson, C. H. (1968). Readability. Stockholm: Liber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., & Bråten, I. (2017). The Discrepancy-induced source comprehension (D-ISC) model: Basic assumptions and preliminary evidence. Educational Psychologist, 52, 167–181. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1323219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Anmarkrud, Ø. (2014). Incremental theories of intelligence predict multiple-documents comprehension. Learning and Individual Differences, 31, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.12.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., Anmarkrud, Ø., & Ferguson, L. E. (2013). Promoting secondary school students’ evaluation of source features of multiple documents. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 180–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.03.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braasch, J. L. G., Rouet, J. F., Vibert, N., & Britt, M. A. (2012). Readers’ use of source information in text comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 40, 450–465. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0160-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Anmarkrud, Ø., Brandmo, C., & Strømsø, H. I. (2014). Developing and testing a model of direct and indirect relationships between individual differences, processing, and multiple-text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 30, 9–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., & Braasch, J. L. G. (2018). The role of conflict in multiple source use. In J. L. G. Braasch, I. Bråten, & M. T. McCrudden (Eds.), Handbook of multiple source use (pp. 184–201). New York: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Braasch, J. L. G., & Salmerón, L. (in press). Reading multiple and non-traditional texts: New opportunities and new challenges. In E. B. Moje, P. Afflerbach, P. Enciso, & N. K. Lesaux (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (vol. V). New York: Routledge.

  • Bråten, I., Brante, E. W., & Strømsø, H. I. (2018a). What really matters: The role of behavioural engagement in multiple document literacy tasks. Journal of Research in Reading, 41, 680–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Gil, L., & Strømsø, H. I. (2011). The role of different task instructions and reader characteristics when learning from multiple expository texts. In M. T. McCrudden, J. P. Magliano, & G. Schraw (Eds.), Text relevance and learning from text (pp. 95–122). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., McCrudden, M. T., Stang Lund, E., Brante, E. W., & Strømsø, H. I. (2018b). Task-oriented learning with multiple documents: Effects of topic familiarity, author expertise, and content relevance on document selection, processing, and use. Reading Research Quarterly, 53, 345–365. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Stadtler, M., & Salmerón, L. (2018c). The role of sourcing in discourse comprehension. In M. F. Schober, D. N. Rapp, & M. A. Britt (Eds.), Handbook of discourse processes (2nd ed., pp. 141–166). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Andreassen, R. (2016). Sourcing in professional education: Do text factors make any difference? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 1599–1628. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9611-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Britt, M. A. (2009). Trust matters: Examining the role of source evaluation in students’ construction of meaning within and across multiple texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 6–28. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.44.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Perfetti, C. A., Sandak, R., & Rouet, J. F. (1999). Content integration and source separation in learning from multiple texts. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative, comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 209–233). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Rouet, J. F., & Braasch, J. L. G. (2013). Documents as entities: Extending the situation model theory of comprehension. In M. A. Britt, S. R. Goldman, & J. F. Rouet (Eds.), Reading: From words to multiple texts (pp. 160–179). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britt, M. A., Rouet, J. F., & Durik, A. (2018). Literacy beyond text comprehension: A theory of purposeful reading. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, B. Y. (2014). Competent adolescent readers’ use of Internet reading strategies: A think-aloud study. Cognition and Instruction, 32, 253–289. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2014.918133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, B. Y., & Afflerbach, P. (2017). An evolving perspective of constructively responsive reading comprehension strategies in multilayered digital text environments. In S. E. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (2nd ed., pp. 109–134). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeStefano, D., & LeFevre, J. (2007). Cognitive load in hypertext reading: A review. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1616–1641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2005.08.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimmons, G., Weal, M. J., & Drieghe, D. (2019). The impact of hyperlinks on reading text. PLoS ONE, 14, e0210900. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, D. P., & Sutton, S. (2010). Reactivity of measurement in health psychology: How much of a problem is it? British Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 453–468. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910710X492341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., Anderson, R., & Tatham, R. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., & Spiro, R. J. (1995). Hypertext learning environments, cognitive flexibility, and the transfer of complex knowledge: An empirical investigation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 12, 301–333. https://doi.org/10.2190/4T1B-HBP0-3F7E-J4PN.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kammerer, Y., & Gerjets, P. (2014). Source evaluations and source references when reading and summarizing science-related information from multiple web pages. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 42, 7–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kammerer, Y., Kalbfell, E., & Gerjets, P. (2016a). Is this information source commercially biased? How contradictions between web pages stimulate the consideration of source information. Discourse Processes, 53, 430–456. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2016.1169968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kammerer, Y., Meier, N., & Stahl, E. (2016b). Fostering secondary-school students’ intertext model formation when reading a set of websites: The effectiveness of source prompts. Computers & Education, 102, 52–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2016.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keck, D., Kammerer, Y., & Starauschek, E. (2015). Reading science texts online: Does source information influence the identification of contradictions within texts? Computers & Education, 82, 442–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerlinger, F. N., & Lee, H. B. (2000). Foundations of behavioral research (4th ed.). Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiili, C., Leu, D. J., Martuunen, M., Hautala, J., & Leppänen, P. (2018). Exploring early adolescents’ evaluation of academic and commercial online resources related to health. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 533–557. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9797-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landow, G. P. (2006). Hypertext 3.0: Critical theory and new media in an era of globalization. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linderholm, T. (2006). Reading with purpose. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 36, 70–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/10790195.2006.10850189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, A., & Alexander, P. A. (2019). Toward an integrated framework of multiple text use. Educational Psychologist, 54, 20–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1505514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magliano, J. P., McCrudden, M. T., Rouet, J. F., & Sabatini, J. (2018). The modern reader: Should changes to how we read affect theory and research? In M. F. Schober, D. N. Rapp, & M. A. Britt (Eds.), Handbook of discourse processes (2nd ed., pp. 343–361). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., Junyent, A. A., & Tornatora, M. C. (2014). Epistemic evaluation and comprehension of web-source information on controversial science-related topics: Effects of a short-term instructional intervention. Computers & Education, 76, 143–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.03.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., Stenseth, T., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2016). The effects of author expertise and content relevance on document selection: A mixed methods study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moan, J., Baturaite, Z., Juzeniene, A., & Porojnicu, A. C. (2012). Vitamin D, sun, sunbeds and health. Public Health Nutrition, 15, 711–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukai, T. O., Bro, F., Fenger-Grøn, M., Olesen, F., & Vedsted, P. (2012). Use of hyperlinks in electronic test result communication: A survey study in general practice. BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making, 12, 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-12-114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naumann, J. (2015). A model of online reading engagement: Linking engagement, navigation, and performance in digital reading. Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 263–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niederhauser, D. S., Reynolds, R. E., Salmen, D. J., & Skolmoski, P. (2000). The influence of cognitive load on learning from hypertext. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 23, 237–255. https://doi.org/10.2190/81BG-RPDJ-9FA0-Q7PA.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, T., Rouet, J. F., François, E., & Zampa, V. (2008). Summarizing digital documents: Effects of alternate or simultaneous window display. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 541–558. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J. F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Towards a theory of documents representation. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 99–122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirolli, P. (2007). Information foraging theory: Adaptive interaction with information. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J. F. (2006). The skills of document use. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rouet, J. F., Le Bigot, L., de Pereyra, G., & Britt, M. A. (2016). Whose story is this? Discrepancy triggers readers’ attention to source information in short narratives. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 1549–1570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9625-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Cañas, J. J., Kintsch, W., & Fajardo, I. (2005). Reading strategies and hypertext comprehension. Discourse Processes, 40, 171–191. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp4003_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., & Garcia, V. (2011). Reading skills and children’s navigation strategies in hypertext. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1143–1151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.12.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Garcia, V., & Vidal Abarca, E. (2018a). The development of adolescents’ comprehension-based Internet reading skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Gil, L., & Bråten, I. (2018b). Effects of reading real versus print-out versions of multiple documents on students’ sourcing and integrated understanding. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 52, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Kammerer, Y., & Delgado, P. (2018c). Non-academic multiple source use on the Internet. In J. L. G. Braasch, I. Bråten, & M. T. McCrudden (Eds.), Handbook of multiple source use (pp. 285–302). New York: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2010). Self-regulation and link selection strategies in hypertext. Discourse Processes, 47, 175–211. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530902728280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Macedo-Rouet, M., & Rouet, J. F. (2016). Multiple viewpoints increase students’ attention to source features in social question and answer forum messages. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 67, 2404–2419. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.23585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Strømsø, H. I., Kammerer, Y., Stadtler, M., & van den Broek, P. (2018d). Comprehension processes in digital reading. In M. Barzillai, J. Thomson, S. Schroeder, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Learning to read in a digital world (pp. 91–120). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saux, G., Britt, M. A., Le Bigot, L., Vibert, N., Burin, D., & Rouet, J. F. (2017). Conflicting but close: Readers’ integration of information sources as a function of their disagreement. Memory and Cognition, 45, 151–167. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0644-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saux, G., Ros, C., Britt, M. A., Stadtler, M., Burin, D. I., & Rouet, J. F. (2018). Readers’ selective recall of source features as a function of claim discrepancy and task demands. Discourse Processes, 55, 525–544. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2018.1463722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scharrer, L., & Salmerón, L. (2016). Sourcing in the reading process: Introduction to the special issue. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 1539–1548. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9676-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., Coulson, R. L., Feltovich, P. J., & Anderson, D. K. (1994). Cognitive flexibility theory: Advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. In R. B. Ruddell, M. R. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 602–615). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., Klautke, H., & Johnson, A. K. (2015). How certain kinds of reading to learn on the Web are totally different from what we learned from research on traditional text comprehension and learning from text. In R. J. Spiro, M. DeSchryver, M. S. Hagerman, P. M. Morsink, & P. Thompson (Eds.), Reading at a crossroads? Disjunctures and continuities in current conceptions and practices (pp. 45–50). New York: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2014). The content-source integration model: A taxonomic description of how readers comprehend conflicting scientific information. In D. N. Rapp & J. L. G. Braasch (Eds.), Processing inaccurate information: Theoretical and applied perspectives from cognitive science and the educational sciences (pp. 379–402). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B., & Bromme, R. (2013). Dealing with uncertainty: Readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as a function of presentation format and source expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 130–150. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2013.769996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Skodzik, T., & Bromme, R. (2014). Comprehending multiple documents on scientific controversies: Effects of reading goals and signaling rhetorical relationships. Discourse Processes, 51, 93–116. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.855535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stang Lund, E., Bråten, I., Brandmo, C., Brante, E. W., & Strømsø, H. I. (2019). Direct and indirect effects of textual and individual factors on source-content integration when reading about a socio-scientific issue. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32, 335–356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9868-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stang Lund, E., Bråten, I., Brante, E. W., & Strømsø, H. I. (2017). Memory for textual conflicts predicts sourcing when adolescents read multiple expository texts. Reading Psychology, 38, 417–437. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2016.1278417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Britt, M. A. (2010). Reading multiple texts about climate change: The relationship between memory for sources and text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 18, 513–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Trahan, M. F. (1992). Learning disabled readers’ comprehension of computer mediated text: The influence of working memory, metacognition, and attribution. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 7, 74–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., & Kendeou, P. (2015). Building coherence in web-based and other non-traditional reading environments: Cognitive opportunities and challenges. In R. J. Spiro, M. DeSchryver, M. S. Hagerman, P. M. Morsink, & P. Thompson (Eds.), Reading at a crossroads? Disjunctures and continuities in current conceptions and practices (pp. 104–114). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinje, F. E. (1982). The journalist language. Fredrikstad: Institute for Journalism.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J. (2001). Supporting understanding through task and browser design. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1136–1143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Wineburg, S. S. (1991). Historical problem solving: A study of the cognitive processes used in the evaluation of documentary and pictorial evidence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 73–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.83.1.73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding for this research was provided by Grants from the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation, and Universities (EDU2017-87626-P) and the European Social Fund to Pablo Delgado and Ladislao Salmerón, and by a travel Grant from the European Cooperation in Science and Technology COST Action IS1404 E-READ to Pablo Delgado. Elisabeth Stang Lund and Ivar Bråten were funded by Grant 237981/H20 from the Research Council of Norway. We would like to thank Guillermo Gómez for his help in programming the website used as experimental materials.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ladislao Salmerón.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix: Title and source information for each of the four texts

Appendix: Title and source information for each of the four texts

Lack of light causes depression

Psychologist Anita Lund

Association for Mental Health

Helsenorge.no*

2016

Too little daylight leads to sleeplessness

Lecturer Kristin Iversen

Norwegian School of Sports Sciences

Olympiatoppen.no**

2013

Sunbathing causes cancer

Chief physician Hilde Dahl

The Norwegian Cancer Association

Journal of Medicine

2014

Vitamin D is important for the body

Nutritionist Nina Sørensen

Norwegian Food Safety Authority

Journal of Food and Health

2015

  1. *A Norwegian public health information website
  2. **The website of the Norwegian Olympic and Paralympic Committee and Confederation of Sports

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Delgado, P., Stang Lund, E., Salmerón, L. et al. To click or not to click: investigating conflict detection and sourcing in a multiple document hypertext environment. Read Writ 33, 2049–2072 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10030-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10030-8

Keywords

Navigation