Skip to main content
Log in

Choices Which Change Life Satisfaction: Similar Results for Australia, Britain and Germany

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using data from national socio-economic panel surveys in Australia, Britain and Germany, this paper analyzes the effects of individual preferences and choices on subjective well-being (SWB). It is shown that, in all three countries, preferences and choices relating to life goals/values, partner’s personality, hours of work, social participation and healthy lifestyle have substantial and similar effects on life satisfaction. The results have negative implications for a widely accepted theory of SWB, set-point theory. This theory holds that adult SWB is stable in the medium and long term, although temporary fluctuations occur due to life events. Set-point theory has come under increasing criticism in recent years, primarily due to unmistakable evidence in the German Socio-Economic Panel that, during the last 25 years, over a third of the population has recorded substantial and apparently permanent changes in life satisfaction (Fujita and Diener in J Pers Soc Psychol 88:158–64, 2005; Headey in Soc Indic Res 85:389–403, 2008a; Headey et al. in Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(42):17922–17926, 2010). It is becoming clear that the main challenge now for SWB researchers is to develop new explanations which can account for medium and long term change, and not merely stability in SWB. Set-point theory is limited precisely because it is purely a theory of stability. The paper is based on specially constructed panel survey files in which data are divided into multi-year periods in order to facilitate analysis of medium and long term change.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Even this degree of stability might not hold in a country with a more turbulent recent history. In the periods in question none of these countries experienced a major war, nor a major economic recession.

  2. See also Hirsch (1976) and Frank (1985) who make a similar distinction between positional and non-positional goods.

  3. The CNEF can be obtained from Cornell University at www.human.cornell.edu\pam\research\centers-program/…/cnef.cfm.

  4. Even the short version of the scale released by Psychological Assessment Resources has 60 items; 12 items per trait (Costa and McCrae 1991).

  5. Ten items were included in 1990, 1992 and 1995 and then nine in 2004 and 2008. The item dropped in 2004 and 2008 related to the importance of having a wide circle of friends, which loaded on the pro-social factor.

  6. For each country response scales relating to social participation have been reversed so that a high score reflects high participation.

  7. The correlations have varied from year to year but are usually around 0.3.

  8. ‘Seldom’ or ‘never’ have been included as separate categories in more recent waves of SOEP.

  9. Many papers only include an age squared term. However, if it is hypothesized that satisfaction declines in middle age and then rises again in one’s senior years, then logically an age cubed term is required as well.

  10. An alternative hypothesis is that ‘unlike poles attract’ and that partners with contrasting personalities will get on better together and have higher SWB. This hypothesis was also tested and rejected via the partner similarity/difference scores constructed here.

  11. Consequently an annual measure of life satisfaction, rather than a 5- or 3-year average measure, serves as the dependent variable.

  12. In Australia the metric regression coefficient for obese women is −0.08 (p < 0.01), in Britain b = −0.21 (p < 0.001) and in Germany b = −0.21 (p < 0.01).

  13. In previous tables, dealing with personality traits and life goals, assumptions required for a fixed effects model were not met. Personality traits have only been measured once in SOEP, and life goals on only a few occasions and at uneven intervals.

  14. The same exceptions apply as in Table 3: German women appear not to mind being overworked and British women do not mind being underworked. Also, in the case of British men, there is no significant link between changes in social participation and changes in life satisfaction (although the coefficient is positive).

  15. These benchmark assessments are made on the basis of re-running analyses with standardized variables and coefficients (Betas). That is, variables were rescaled to have means of zero and standard deviations of one. Rough comparisons can then be made between the effect sizes of regression coefficients, because they have all had the same metric imposed.

References

  • Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation level theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. R. (1976). The quality of american life. New York: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y., & Lucas, R. E. (2008). Lags and leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. Economic Journal, 118, 222–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influences of extroversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1991). The NEO PI-R. Odessa, FL: PAR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of psychological wealth. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. Psychological Science, 61, 305–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 25, 276–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science, 319, 1687–1688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (2005). Building a better theory of well-being. In L. Bruni & P. Porta (Eds.), Economics and happiness: Framing the analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebrahim, S., & Smith, G. D. (1997). Systematic review of randomized control trials of multiple risk factor interventions for preventing coronary heart disease. British Medical Journal, 314, 1666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erhardt, J. J., Saris, W. E., & Veenhoven, R. (2000). Life-satisfaction over time: Analysis of changes in ranks in a national population. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1, 177–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1985). The demand for unobservable and other nonpositional goods. American Economic Review, 75, 279–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 402–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., Haisken-DeNew, J. P., & Shields, M. A. (2004) Money does matter! Evidence from increasing real incomes in east Germany following reunification. American Economic Review, 94, 730–741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set-point: Stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholgy, 88, 158–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerlitz, J.-Y. & Schupp, J. (2005) Zur Erhebung der Big-Five-basierten Persoenlichkeitsmerkmale im SOEP. www.diw.de/deutsche/produkte/publikationen/researchnotes/docs/papers/rn4.pdf.

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Hidajat, M., Mayraz, G., Lindenberger, U., Schupp, J., et al. (2010). Late-life decline in well-being across adulthood in Germany, the UK, and the US: Something is seriously wrong at the end of life. Psychology and Aging, 25, 477–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2010). Personal reflections on theory and psychology. Theory and Psychology, 20, 733–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1997). The marriage clinic: A scientifically-based marital therapy. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, R. E., & Cantor, N. (1996). Still participating after all these years: A study of life task participation in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1235–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W. (2006). Subjective well-being: Revisions to dynamic equilibrium theory using national panel data and panel regression methods. Social Indicators Research, 79, 369–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W. (2008a). The set-point theory of well-being: Negative results and consequent revisions. Social Indicators Research, 85, 389–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W. (2008b). Life goals matter to happiness: A revision of set-point theory. Social Indicators Research, 86, 213–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W., Muffels, R. J. A., & Wagner, G. G. (2010). Long-running German panel survey shows that personal and economic choices, not just genes, matter for happiness. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(42), 17922–17926 (Oct. 19).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W., & Wearing, A. J. (1989). Personality, life events and subjective well-being: Towards a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, F. (1976). Social limits to growth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huppert, F. (2005). Positive mental health in individuals and populations. In F. Huppert, N. Baylis, & B. Keverne (Eds.), The science of well-being (pp. 307–340). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Kanner, A. D. (Eds.). (2004). Psychology and consumer culture: The struggle for the good life in a materialistic world. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluckhohn, F. R., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Variations in value orientations. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J. (1992). Neuroticism and selective encoding and recall of symptoms: Evidence from a combined concurrent-retrospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 489–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2007). Long-term disability is associated with lasting changes in subjective well-being: Evidence from two nationally representative longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 717–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2008). Personality and subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 171–194). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Baird, B. M. (2004). Extraversion and emotional reactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 473–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to change in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). How stable is happiness? Using the STARTS model to estimate the stability of life satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1091–1098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. (1999). Happiness: What studies on twins show us about nature, nurture and the happiness set-point. New York: Golden Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, P. (2006). Quality profile: BHPS version 2.0: Waves 1 to 13 1991–2003. Essex: University of Essex, Institute for Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). The how of happiness: A scientific approach to getting the life you want. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehnert, T., Kraus, H. H., Nadler, R., & Boyd, M. (1990). Correlates of life satisfaction in those with a disabling condition. Rehabilitation Psychology, 35, 3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, C., Schwarz, N., Diener, E., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Zeroing in on the dark side of the American dream: A closer look at the negative consequences of the goal for financial success. Psychological Science, 14, 531–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 3–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2000). Two personalities, one relationship: Both partners’ personality traits shape the quality of their relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 251–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1997). Extraversion, neuroticism and susceptibility to positive and negative affect. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 607–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: A brief version of Goldberg’s Big Five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 506–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Environmental influences on subjective well-being: A dyadic latent panel analysis of spousal similarity. Social Indicators Research, 98, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1152–1165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thoits, P. A., & Hewitt, L. N. (2001). Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, G. G., Frick, J. R., & Schupp, J. (2007). The German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP)—Scope, evolution and enhancements. Schmollers Jahrbuch, 127(1), 139–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, N., & Wooden, M. (2004). Assessing the quality of the HILDA survey wave 2 data. Melbourne: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkelmann, R. (2004) Subjective well-being and the family: Results from an ordered probit model with multiple random effects. IZA discussion paper no. 1016.

  • Wortman, C. B., & Silver, R. C. (1987). Coping with irrevocable loss. In G. R. Vanderbos & B. K. Bryant (Eds.), Cataclysms, crises, catastrophes: Psychology in action. Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Ed Diener of the University of Illinois, Daniel Kahneman of Princeton University, Andrew Oswald of Warwick University and Alexander Wearing of Melbourne University for insightful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruce Headey.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Headey, B., Muffels, R. & Wagner, G.G. Choices Which Change Life Satisfaction: Similar Results for Australia, Britain and Germany. Soc Indic Res 112, 725–748 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0079-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0079-8

Keywords

Navigation