Skip to main content
Log in

One Size Does Not Fit All: Alternative Values-Based ‘Recipes’ for Life Satisfaction

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In most previous research on the determinants of Life Satisfaction (LS), there has been an implicit assumption that ‘one size fits all’. That is, it has usually been assumed that the covariates of LS are the same for everyone, or at least everyone in the Western world. In this paper, using data from the long-running German Socio-Economic Panel (1984-), we estimate statistical models to assess the effects of commitment to differing personal values on LS. The personal values in question are: traditional family values, pro-social altruistic values and materialistic (money and career) values. These values are linked to differing behavioural choices with substantial effects on domain satisfactions and LS. It can be inferred that linked sets of values, behavioural choices and domain satisfactions may constitute alternative ‘recipes’ for LS. However, in the absence of direct evidence of motivation, it is not claimed that panel respondents consciously follow these ‘recipes’ with a view to enhancing their LS. Our results indicate that the effect of individuals adhering to a traditional family values ‘recipe’ or an altruistic values ‘recipe’ is that they record above average LS, whereas materialistic values are linked to below average LS.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1974) theory of reasoned action has provided a framework for much of the debate. Their focus is more on the attitude-behaviour link, rather than values and behaviour, but values may be regarded as one type of attitude.

  2. They hypothesised that they would find statistically significant interactions between satisfaction with particular life domains and the importance attached to those domains, such that people who were highly satisfied in life domains that they rated as ‘important’ would record increased levels of LS, over and above the levels predicted by the domain satisfactions alone. No significant interactions were found.

  3. An earlier ‘discussion paper’ version of this article is Headey and Wagner (2018).

  4. We thank SIR reviewers for pointing out that a previous version of the paper could have been taken to imply that personal values are held primarily with a view to promoting one’s own LS.

  5. Household income is not included in our model of materialist values because it is likely to be endogenous; i.e. partly a consequence rather than a cause of materialist values. ‘Unemployed’ is also excluded from models of materialist values because the focus of these models is largely on the consequences of values for working hours and individual labour income (see below).

  6. We use the terms ‘pro-social’ and ‘altruistic’ to refer to values which favour behaving cooperatively and helping others. In evolutionary biology, altruism is defined as behaviour which involves a loss/cost to the individual concerned, as well as gains to others; we do not imply that here.

  7. This scale has been reversed so that a high rating means ‘very important’.

  8. The variable included here measures the number of children a person has ever had (not the number currently living in the household).

  9. Respondents are asked to estimate time spent per week on various activities. This approach to measurement is not as accurate as the daily diary method of collecting time use data. However, it has generally been found to be adequate for producing rank order data, comparing the time uses of different population groups (Juster et al. 2003).

  10. This was first measured in SOEP in 2006 and has been measured annually since.

  11. Satisfaction with volunteering was only measured in 2007 and 2009. Satisfaction with social life has been measured since 2006.

  12. We used satisfaction with household income, rather than personal income, in our estimations because the former question has been asked every year in SOEP, while the latter has only been asked intermittently. However, results were very similar, regardless of the question included.

  13. ML estimates are usually consistent and asymptotically normal under the (not very restrictive) assumption of conditional normality (StataCorp 2017). Only paths or covariances linking conditioning (i.e. control) variables may not be consistent and asymptotically normal (even then, the main problem lies just with estimates of standard errors). These paths are not usually of substantive interest. Substantive interest lies in paths (1) linking exogenous with endogenous variables and (2) between endogenous variables.

  14. We also tried some ‘mixed’ combinations of time lags. For example, we ran models with zero lags between socio-economic variables, personality traits and values… then one-year lags between values and behavioural choices…and one year lags between choices and domain satisfactions. These ‘mixed’ lag models also fitted the input data less satisfactorily than models with zero lags.

  15. The inclusion of lagged versions of the independent and dependent variables reduces, but does not completely eliminate, the potential biasing effects of omitted variables (Finkel 1995; Wooldridge 2010).

  16. However, there is still a risk of biased estimates due to omitted variables which change over time within-person.

  17. The difference appears to be just additive. No interaction effect was found linking this pair of variables to LS.

  18. This latter variable was not included in the probit equation described above, because data are only available for 2015–16, rather than 1990–2016.

  19. Covariances between exogeneous variables and between error terms of endogenous variables are also omitted.

  20. In other words, the difference between the actual input variance-covariance matrix and the matrix predicted by this family values model is not statistically significant.

  21. The Stata software prints out direct, indirect and total effects routinely (StataCorp. 2017).

  22. This simply involved inserting a measure of partners’ family values on the RHS of equations that were otherwise the same as reported in Fig. 2 and Appendix Table 4.

  23. Only the 25% of respondents who report doing some volunteering would be left in the model.

  24. Costa and McCrae (1991), the authors of the NEO personality inventory, list cooperativeness as one of the sub-traits related to agreeableness.

  25. The coefficient of 1.22 linking job satisfaction and materialistic values is not significantly different from coefficients linking job satisfaction with altruistic values or family values. Only people who prioritise none of these values have significantly lower job satisfaction (i.e. the ‘Nothing valued’ group).

  26. In each wave respondents are asked their preferred hours of work, as well as their actual hours.

  27. This result also holds for people in the top 5% (two standard deviations above the mean) of the earnings distribution.

References

  • Adler, M.D., Dolan, P. & Kavestos, G. (2015). Would you choose to be happy? Trade-offs between happiness and other dimensions of life in a large population survey, Duke Law School Public Law and Legal Theory Series, No. 2015–35.

  • Aguche, A. & Trommsdorff, G. (2010). Transmission of well-being between mothers, fathers and adolescent children: the role of parenting and personality factors, 9th International German Socio-Economic Panel Users Conference, June 30–July 1.

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (2002). Obituary, Daily Telegraph (London) (September 12).

  • Benjamin, D. J., Heffez, O., Kimball, M., & Rees-Jones, A. (2010). Do people seek to maximize happiness? Evidence from new surveys. Mimeo: Cornell University.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beytia, P. (2015). The singularity of latin American patterns of happiness. In M. Rojas (Ed.), Handbook of happiness research in latin America (pp. 17–29). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Konow, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). Materialism, spending and affect: An event-sampling study of marketplace behaviour and its affective costs. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 2277–2292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, N., & Sanderson, C. A. (1999). ‘Life task participation and well-being: The importance of taking part in daily life’ in. In D. Kahenman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. thousand (pp. 240–243). Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concerns. New Brunswick: Rutgers Univ Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corporation, Stata. (2017). Stata: Structural equation reference manual, release 15. Texas: College Station.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1991). The NEO PI-R. Odessa: PAR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health and well-being around the world: Evidence from the gallup world poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22, 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of psychological wealth. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926–935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13, 81–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 25, 276–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth, A. L., Weir, D., Tsukayama, E., & Kwok, D. (2012). Who does well in life? Conscientious adults excel in both objective and subjective success. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science, 319, 1687–1688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract versus concrete goals: Personal striving level, physical illness, and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 292–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict among personal strivings: Immediate and long-term implications for psychological and physical well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1040–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, S. E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes towards objects as predictors of single and multiple behavioral criteria. Psychological Review, 81, 59–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2009). Positivity. New York: Crown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60(7), 678–686. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 402–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, J. R., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2007). Enhancing the power of the German Socio- Economic Panel Study (SOEP)—evolution, scope and enhancements. Schmollers Jahrbuch, 127, 139–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., Haisken-DeNew, J. P., & Shields, M. A. (2004). Money does matter! Evidence from increasing real income and life satisfaction in East Germany following reunification. American Economic Review, 94, 730–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goebel, J., Grabka, M., Liebig, S., Kroh, M., Richter, D., Schroeder, C., et al. (2018). The German socio-economic panel study (SOEP). Jahrbuecher für Nationalökonomie und Statistik/Journal of Economics and Statistics (online first). https://doi.org/10.1515/jbnst-2018-0022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, R. E., & Cantor, N. (1996). Still participating after all these years: A study of life task participation in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1235–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W. (2008). Life goals matter to happiness: A revision of set-point theory. Social Indicators Research, 86, 213–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B.W. & Muffels, R.J.A. (2015) Towards a theory of medium term Life Satisfaction: Two-way causation partly explains persistent satisfaction or dissatisfaction, Social Indicators Research, 124.2. Published online 14 October 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-1146-8.

  • Headey, B. W., & Muffels, R. J. A. (2018). A theory of Life satisfaction dynamics: Stability, change and volatility in 25-year life trajectories in Germany. Social Indicators Research, 140, 837–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W., Muffels, R. J. A., & Wagner, G. G. (2010). Long-running German panel survey shows that personal and economic choices, not just genes, matter for happiness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107.42, 17922–17926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B. W., & Wagner, G. G. (2018). Alternative values-based ‘recipes’ for life satisfaction: German results with an Australian replication, IZA Discussion Paper No 11935. IZA: Bonn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R. F., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom and rising happiness: A global perspective 1981-2006. Perspectives on psychological science, 3, 264–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juster, F. T., Hiromi, O., & Stafford, F. P. (2003). An assessment of alternative measures of time use. Sociological Methodology, 33, 19–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluckhohn, F. R., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Variations in value orientations. Illinois: Evanston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2008). Happiness: Lessons from a new science (2nd ed.). Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E. (2008). Personality and subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 171–194). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). The how of happiness. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ng, W., & Diener, E. (2014). What matters to the rich and the poor? Subjective well-being, financial satisfaction and postmaterialist needs across the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 326–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, C., Schwarz, N., Diener, E., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Zeroing in on the dark side of the American dream: A closer look at the negative consequences of the goal for financial success. Psychological Science, 14, 531–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, D., Metzing, M., Weinhardt, M., & Schupp, J. (2013). SOEP scales manual (p. 138). No: SOEP Survey Papers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, J. M., Richter, D., Brummer, M., Wagner, G. G., & Schmukle, S. C. (2018). Successfully striving for happiness: Socially engaged pursuits predict increases in Life Satisfaction. Psychological Science, 29, 1291–1298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rojas, M. (2018). ‘Happiness in Latin America has social foundations’, United Nations. New York, United Nations Sustainable Development Solutions Network: World Happiness Report. chapter 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent Variables Analysis: Applications for Developmental Research. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, K. C. (2017). An investigation into the stability of the big-five in germany. Berlin: SOEP Technical Report.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Environmental influences on well-being: A dyadic latent panel analysis of spousal similarity. Social Indicators Research, 98, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, S.H. (2012). An overview of the Schwarz theory of basic values. Online readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1) https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1116.

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourishing. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoits, P. A., & Hewitt, L. N. (2001). Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations (2012-17) World Happiness Reports. New York, United Nations Sustainable Development Solutions Network.

  • Veenhoven, R. (2012). Does happiness differ across cultures? In H. Selin & G. Davey (Eds.), Happiness across cultures: The history of non-western science 6 (pp. 451–472). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2018 regularly updated) World Database of Happiness: Archive of Research Findings on Subjective Enjoyment of Life. Erasmus University Rotterdam, Happiness Economics Research Organization. Accessed January 9 2019.

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 370–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wooldridge, J. M. (2010). Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data (2nd ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruce Headey.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix: Traditional Family Values Model: Full Model Estimates

Appendix: Traditional Family Values Model: Full Model Estimates

Appendix Table 4 (like Appendix Tables 5 and 6 below) gives results in four panels, reflecting the four steps in the model. Panel 1 shows the effects on family values of socio-economic variables and personality traits. Panel 2 gives the effects on behavioural choices of socio-economic variables, traits and family values. In panel 3 the outcome variable is a domain satisfaction—satisfaction with family life—and the explanatory variables are socio-economic characteristics, traits, values and behavioural choices. In panel 4 the outcome variable is LS with variance being accounted for by socio-economic variables, traits, values, choices and domain satisfactions.

Table 4 Traditional family values model: a structural equation model (N = 66,302)
Table 5 Pro-social altruistic values model: A structural equation model (N = 69,050)
Table 6 Materialistic values model: a structural equation model (N = 56,648)

Data analysed in the traditional family values model, and also the altruistic values and materialistic values models, are for 2006–16 and relate to respondents in the 25–54 age range.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Headey, B., Wagner, G.G. One Size Does Not Fit All: Alternative Values-Based ‘Recipes’ for Life Satisfaction. Soc Indic Res 145, 581–613 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-019-02108-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-019-02108-w

Keywords

Navigation