Skip to main content
Log in

To Each According to his Need? Variability in the Responses to Inequity in Non-Human Primates

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While it is well established that humans respond to inequity, it remains unclear the extent to which this behavior occurs in our non-human primate relatives. By comparing a variety of species, spanning from New World and Old World monkeys to great apes, scientists can begin to answer questions about how the response to inequity evolved, what the function of this response is, and why and how different contexts shape it. In particular, research across non-human primate species suggests that the response is quite variable across species, contexts, and individuals. In this paper, we aim to review these differences in an attempt to identify and better understand the patterns that emerge from the existing data with the goal of developing directions for future research. To begin, we address the importance of considering socio-ecological factors in non-human primates in order to better understand and predict expected patterns of cooperation and aversion to inequity in different species, following which we provide a detailed analysis of the patterns uncovered by these comparisons. Ultimately, we use this synthesis to propose new ideas for research to better understand this response and, hence, the evolution of our own responses to inequity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abeyesinghe, S., Nicol, C., Hartnell, S., & Wathes, C. (2005). Can domestic fowl, Gallus gallus domesticus, show self-control? Animal Behaviour, 70(1), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Addessi, E., Crescimbene, L., & Visalberghi, E. (2007). Do capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use tokens as symbols? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274(1625), 2579.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Addessi, E., & Rossi, S. (2011). Tokens improve capuchin performance in the reverse–reward contingency task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278(1707), 849–854.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A. (1992). Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging Orangutans. Child Development, 63(5), 1186–1197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2004). Wild justice, cooperation, and fair play: Minding manners, being nice, and feeling good. In R. Sussman & A. Chapman (Eds.), The Origins and Nature of Sociality (pp. 53–79). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beran, M. J., & Evans, T. A. (2006). Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effects of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behavioural Processes, 73, 315–324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1994). Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 48(3), 653–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C., & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Tai Forest: Behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski, S. (1987). Birth synchrony in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedi). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 21(6), 393–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski, S. (1988). Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper). American Journal of Primatology, 14(2), 177–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski, S. (1999). The social organizations of squirrel monkeys: Implications for ecological models of social evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 8(3), 101–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273(1605), 3123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71(2), 175–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., & Hanus, D. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Social Justice Research, 19(2), 153–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2010). What do capuchin monkeys tell us about cooperation? In D. R. Forsyth & C. L. Hoyt (Eds.), For the greater good of all: Perspectives on individualism, society, and leadership (Vol. Jepson studies in leadership series) (pp. 11–28). Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2011). A hypothesis of the co-evolution of cooperation and responses to inequity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2012). Introduction to justice in animals. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0156-9.

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425(6955), 297–299.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. (2004a). Socially learned preferences for differentially rewarded tokens in the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(2), 133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2004b). A concept of value during experimental exchange in brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Folia Primatologica, 75, 317–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Brosnan, S. F., Flemming, T., Talbot, C. F., Mayo, L., & Stoinski, T. (2011). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) do not form expectations based on their partner’s outcomes. Folia Primatologica, 82(1), 56–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Freeman, C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Partner’s behavior, not reward distribution, determines success in an unequal cooperative task in capuchin monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 68(7), 713–724.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Houser, D., Leimgruber, K., Xiao, E., Chen, T., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Competing demands of prosociality and equity in monkeys. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(4), 279–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Parrish, A., Beran, M. J., Flemming, T., Heimbauer, L., Talbot, C. F., et al. (2011). Responses to the assurance game in monkeys, apes, and humans using equivalent procedures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(8), 3442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Schiff, H., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 272(1560), 253.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Talbot, C., Ahlgren, M., Lambeth, S. P., & Schapiro, S. J. (2010). Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), 1229–1237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Wilson, B., & Beran, M. J. (2012). Old World monkeys are more similar to humans than New World monkeys when playing a coordination game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 1522–1530.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. R., Almond, R. E. A., & Bates, N. J. (2005). Adult–infant food transfer in common marmosets: An experimental study. American Journal of Primatology, 65(4), 301–312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. R., Almond, R. E. A., & Bergen, Y. (2004). Begging, stealing, and offering: Food transfer in nonhuman primates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 34, 265–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(50), 19762.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S., & Van Schaik, C. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 175–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2010). Primate social cognition: Uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron, 65(6), 815–830.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., & Stumpf, R. M. (2010). Primates in perspective (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmeau, R., Lardeux, K., Brandibas, P., & Gallo, A. (1997). Cooperative problem solving by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). International Journal of Primatology, 18(1), 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, J., & Houser, D. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Chen, M. K., & Santos, L. R. (2006). Some thoughts on the adaptive function of inequity aversion: An alternative to Brosnan’s social hypothesis. Social Justice Research, 19(2), 201–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christen, M., & Glock, H. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Cowlishaw, G., & Dunbar, R. I. M. D. (2000). Primate conservation biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, K. A., Kurian, A. V., & Snowdon, C. T. (2005). Cooperative problem solving in a cooperatively breeding primate (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Behaviour, 69(1), 133–142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, K. A., Schroeder, K. K. E., Rothwell, E. S., Silk, J. B., & Snowdon, C. T. (2009). Cooperatively breeding cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) do not donate rewards to their long-term mates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(3), 231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, K. A., Schroeder, K. K. E., & Snowdon, C. T. (2010). Prosocial behaviour emerges independent of reciprocity in cottontop tamarins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277(1701), 3845–3851.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, K. A., & Snowdon, C. T. (2008). The effects of unequal reward distributions on cooperative problem solving by cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Animal Behaviour, 75(1), 245–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1977). The organization of agonistic relations within two captive groups of java-monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 44(3), 225–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1992). Appeasement, celebration, and food sharing in the two Pan species. Topics in primatology, 1, 37–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1995). Bonobo sex and society. Scientific American, 272(3), 82–88.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1997). Food transfers through mesh in brown capuchins. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(4), 370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1997a). The chimpanzee’s service economy: food for grooming. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(6), 375–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & Berger, M. L. (2000). Payment for labour in monkeys. Nature, 404(6778), 563.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., Leimgruber, K., & Greenberg, A. R. (2008). Giving is self-rewarding for monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(36), 13685.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deaner, R. O., Isler, K., Burkart, J., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 70(2), 115–124.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dindo, M., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Partner effects on food consumption in brown capuchin monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 69(4), 448–456.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dufour, V., Pelé, M., Neumann, M., Thierry, B., & Call, J. (2009). Calculated reciprocity after all: Computation behind token transfers in orangutans. Biology Letters, 5(2), 172–175.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dufour, V., Pelé, M., Sterck, E., & Thierry, B. (2007). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) anticipation of food return: Coping with waiting time in an exchange task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(2), 145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, S. D., & Snowdon, C. T. (1980). Social behavior of captive, group-living orangutans. International Journal of Primatology, 1(1), 39–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2007a). Delay of gratification and delay maintenance by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). The Journal of General Psychology, 134(2), 199–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2007b). Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biology Letters, 3(6), 599–602.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, T. A., & Westergaard, G. C. (2006). Self-control and tool use in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(2), 163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feistner, A. T. C., & Price, E. C. (1999). Cross-generic food sharing in tamarins. International Journal of Primatology, 20(2), 231–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feistner, A. T. C., & Price, E. C. (2000). Food sharing in black lion tamarins (Leontopithecus chrysopygus). American Journal of Primatology, 52(1), 47–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, E., Rotundo, M., & Sloan, C. (2001). Density and population structure of owl monkeys (Aotus azarai) in the Argentinean Chaco. American Journal of Primatology, 53(3), 99–108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, G. E. (2008). Attending to the outcome of others: Disadvantageous inequity aversion in male capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology, 70(9), 901–905.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fontenot, M., Watson, S., Roberts, K., & Miller, R. (2007). Effects of food preferences on token exchange and behavioural responses to inequality in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 74(3), 487–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fragaszy, D. M., Visalberghi, E., & Fedigan, L. M. (2004). The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. New york: Cambridge University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: WW Norton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (2001). Cooperation through emotional commitment. Evolution and the capacity for commitment, 3, 57–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, H. D., Sullivan, J., Schultz-Darken, N., Williams, L. E., & Brosnan, S. F (in review). All in the family? Cooperative breeders’ response to inequity provides support for the cooperative breeder hypothesis. Biology Letters.

  • Galdikas, B. M. F. (1988). Orangutan diet, range, and activity at Tanjung Puting, Central Borneo. International Journal of Primatology, 9(1), 1–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garber, P. A., Moya, L., & Malaga, C. (1984). A preliminary field study of the moustached tamarin monkey (Saguinus mystax) in northeastern Peru: Questions concerned with the evolution of a communal breeding system. Folia Primatologica, 42(1), 17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldizen, A. W. (1987). Tamarins and marmosets: Communal care of offspring. In B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 34–43). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomes, C. M., & Boesch, C. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLoS ONE, 4(4), e5116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros-Louis, J., Perry, S., & Manson, J. H. (2003). Violent coalitionary attacks and intraspecific killing in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Primates, 44(4), 341–346.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gumert, M. D. (2007a). Grooming and infant handling interchange in Macaca fascicularis: The relationship between infant supply and grooming payment. International Journal of Primatology, 28(5), 1059–1074.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gumert, M. D. (2007b). Payment for sex in a macaque mating market. Animal Behaviour, 74(6), 1655–1667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gumert, M. D., & Ho, M. H. R. (2008). The trade balance of grooming and its coordination of reciprocation and tolerance in Indonesian long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Primates, 49(3), 176–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H. (1978). Strategies of emigration and transfer by primates, with particular reference to gorillas. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 48(4), 401–420. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1978.tb00267.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H. (1979a). Social relationships between adult male and female mountain gorillas in the wild. Animal Behaviour, 27, 325–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H. (1979b). Social relationships among adult female mountain gorillas. Animal Behaviour, 27, 251–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H., & Stewart, K. J. (2007). Gorilla society: Conflict, compromise and cooperation between the sexes. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H., & de Waal, F. B. M. (1992). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59(4), 771–785.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., & Wrangham, R. (2007). Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Current Biology, 17(7), 619–623.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56(1), 51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hattori, Y., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2005). Cooperative problem solving by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Spontaneous division of labor, communication, and reciprocal altruism. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(3), 335.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. K., Laere, G. J., & Hooff, J. A. (1992). Sexual exchange relationships in captive chimpanzees? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 30(3), 269–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockings, K. J., Humle, T., Anderson, J. R., Biro, D., Sousa, C., Ohashi, G., et al. (2007). Chimpanzees share forbidden fruit. PLoS ONE, 2(9), e886.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., & Fruth, B. (1993). Field observations on meat sharing among bonobos (Pan paniscus). Folia Primatologica, 60(4), 225–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., Bernacky, B. J., & Brosnan, S. F (in review). The ontogeny of social comparisons by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Developmental Science.

  • Horowitz, A. (2012). Fair is fine, but more is better: Limits to inequity aversion in the domestic dog. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0158-7.

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Idani, G. (1991). Social relationships between immigrant and resident bonobo (Pan paniscus) females at Wamba. Folia Primatologica, 57(2), 83–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V., Stevens, J. M. G., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Tolerant food sharing and reciprocity is precluded by despotism among bonobos but not chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 143(1), 41–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V., van Noordwijk, M. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Begging for information: Mother–offspring food sharing among wild Bornean orangutans. American Journal of Primatology, 70(6), 533–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H., & Brosnan, S. F. (in review). Experimental methods in primatology. In E. Sterling, N. Bynum & M. Blair (Eds.), Primate Ecology and Conservation: A Handbook of Techniques. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Jerison, H. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kano, T. (1982). The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba. Primates, 23(2), 171–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kano, T. (1992). The last ape: Pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Ann Arbor, MI: Univ Microfilms Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A. (1995). Group size, composition, and reproductive success in wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 35(4), 311–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuroda, S. (1984). Interaction over food among pygmy chimpanzees. In R. L. Susman (Ed.), The pygmy chimpanzee, evolutionary biology and behaviour (pp. 301–324). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakshminarayanan, V. R., & Santos, L. R. (2008). Capuchin monkeys are sensitive to others’ welfare. Current Biology, 18(21), R999–R1000.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, D. H., & Festinger, L. (1962). Deterrents and reinforcement: The psychology of insufficient reward. Stanford, KY: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazaro-Perea, C. (2001). Intergroup interactions in wild common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus: Territorial defence and assessment of neighbours. Animal Behaviour, 62(1), 11–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, J. (1974). The behaviour and ecology of wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Behaviour, 22(1), 3–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maestripieri, D., Ross, S. K., & Megna, N. L. (2002). Mother–infant interactions in western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): Spatial relationships, communication and opportunities for social learning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116(3), 219.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Ferrario, A., Valsecchi, P., & Prato-Previde, E. (2011). Social eavesdropping in the domestic dog. Animal Behaviour, 81, 1177–1183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. D. (1984). Body size, brain size and feeding strategies. In D. J. Chivers, B. A. Wood, & A. Bilsborough (Eds.), Food acquisition and processing in primates (pp. 73–103). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J. J. M. (2010). ‘Friendship’ in macaques: Economics and emotions. PhD thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht.

  • Massen, J. J. M., Luyten, I. J. A. F., Spruijt, B. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2011). Benefiting friends or dominants: Prosocial choices mainly depend on rank position in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Primates, 52, 237–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J. J. M., Van Den Berg, L. M., Spruijt, B. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2010). Generous leaders and selfish underdogs: Pro-sociality in despotic macaques. PLoS ONE, 5(3), e9734.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J. J. M., van den Berg, L. M., Spruijt, B. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2011). Inequity aversion in relation to effort and relationship quality in long-tailed Macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology, 74(2), 145–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. 5. The Effect of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006a). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science, 311(5765), 1297–1300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006b). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72(2), 275–286. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., & Watts, D. P. (2001). Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 915–924.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, C. L. (1994). Migration alliances and coalitions among adult male South American squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Behaviour, 130, 169–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neiworth, J. J., Johnson, E. T., Whillock, K., Greenberg, J., & Brown, V. (2009). Is a sense of inequity an ancestral primate trait? Testing social inequity in cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(1), 10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, T., Hasegawa, T., Hayaki, H., Takahata, Y., & Uehara, S. (1992). Meat-sharing as a coalition strategy by an alpha male chimpanzee. Topics in primatology, 1, 159–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, K. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Parish, A. R. (1994). Sex and food control in the “uncommon chimpanzee”: How Bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of male dominance. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(3), 157–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parish, A. R. (1996). Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Human Nature, 7(1), 61–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, S. (1998). Male–male social relationships in wild white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour, 135, 139–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, S., & Rose, L. (1994). Begging and transfer of coati meat by white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Primates, 35(4), 409–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petit, O., Desportes, C., & Thierry, B. (1992). Differential probability of “coproduction” in two species of macaque (Macaca tonkeana, M. mulatta). Ethology, 90(2), 107–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, J. & Bekoff, M. (2012). Wild justice redux: What we know about social justice in animals and why it matters. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0154-y.

  • Preuschoft, S., Wang, X., Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Reconciliation in captive chimpanzees: A reevaluation with controlled methods. International Journal of Primatology, 23(1), 29–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, E. C., & Feistner, A. T. C. (2001). Food sharing in pied bare-faced tamarins (Saguinus bicolor bicolor): Development and individual differences. International Journal of Primatology, 22(2), 231–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pruetz, J., & Lindshield, S. (2011). Plant-food and tool transfer among savanna chimpanzees at Fongoli, Senegal. Primates, 53, 133–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raihani, N.J., & McAuliffe, K. (2012). Does inequity aversion motivate punishment? Cleaner fish as a model system. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0157-8.

  • Range, F., Leitner, K., & Virányi, Z. (2012). The influence of the relationship and motivation on inequity aversion in dogs. Social Justice Research, 25(2). doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0155-x.

  • Rapaport, L. G. (1999). Provisioning of young in golden lion tamarins (Callitrichidae, Leontopithecus rosalia): A test of the information hypothesis. Ethology, 105(7), 619–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, L. G. (2006). Provisioning in wild golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia): Benefits to omnivorous young. Behavioral Ecology, 17(2), 212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, L. G., & Ruiz-Miranda, C. R. (2002). Tutoring in wild golden lion tamarins. International Journal of Primatology, 23(5), 1063–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1567), 1017–1027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, J. B., Mitchell, S. H., De Wit, H., & Seiden, L. S. (1997). Determination of discount functions in rats with an adjusting-amount procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67(3), 353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, M. M. (1995). A demographic analysis of male life history and social structure of mountain gorillas. Behaviour, 132, 21–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roma, P. G., Silberberg, A., Ruggiero, A. M., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Capuchin monkeys, inequity aversion, and the frustration effect. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 67.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosati, A., Hare, B., & Santos, L. (2010). Primate social cognition: Thirty years after Premack and Woodruff. Primate Neuroethology, 1(9), 117–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, L. M. (1994). Benefits and costs of resident males to females in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. American Journal of Primatology, 32(4), 235–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, L. M. (1997). Vertebrate predation and food-sharing in Cebus and Pan. International Journal of Primatology, 18(5), 727–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, G., & Dicke, U. (2012). Evolution of the brain and intelligence in primates. In M. A. Hofman & D. Falk (Eds.), Evolution of the Primate Brain: From Neuron to Behavior (1st ed., pp. 413–430). Oxford: Elsevier Science Ltd.

  • Rotundo, M., Fernandez-Duque, E., & Dixson, A. F. (2005). Infant development and parental care in free-ranging Aotus azarai azarai in Argentina. International Journal of Primatology, 26(6), 1459–1473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotundo, M., Fernandez-Duque, E., & Gimenez, M. (2002). Biparental care in the night monkey (Aotus azarai) of Formosa, Argentina. Neotropical Primates, 10, 70–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage, A., Snowdon, C. T., Giraldo, L. H., & Soto, L. H. (1996). Parental care patterns and vigilance in wild cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). In M. A. Norconk, A. L. Rosenberger, & P. A. Garber (Eds.), Adaptive radiations of neotropical primates (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, G. E. (1963). The mountain gorilla: Ecology and behavior. Oxford: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberberg, A., Crescimbene, L., Addessi, E., Anderson, J. R., & Visalberghi, E. (2009). Does inequity aversion depend on a frustration effect? A test with capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 12(3), 505–509.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B. (1978). Patterns of food sharing among mother and infant chimpanzees at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 29(2), 129–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Brosnan, S. F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D. J., Richardson, A. S., et al. (2005). Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 437(7063), 1357–1359.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skitka, L. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., & Struhsaker, T. T. (1987). Primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. (1996). Infant care in cooperatively breeding species. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 25, 643–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. (1967). Small group ecology. Psychological Bulletin, 67(2), 145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sousa, C., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001). The use of tokens as rewards and tools by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 4(3), 213–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. R. (2004). The selfish nature of generosity: Harassment and food sharing in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 271(1538), 451.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. R. (2010). Donor payoffs and other-regarding preferences in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 13(4), 663–670.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. R., Rosati, A. G., Ross, K. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Will travel for food: Spatial discounting in two new world monkeys. Current Biology, 15(20), 1855–1860.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sussman, R., & Garber, P. (1987). A new interpretation of the social organization and mating system of the Callitrichidae. International Journal of Primatology, 8(1), 73–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szalda-Petree, A. D., Craft, B. B., Martin, L. M., & Deditius-Island, H. K. (2004). Self-control in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): Controlling for differential stimulus exposure. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98(1), 141–146. doi:10.2466/pms.98.1.141-146.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takimoto, A., & Fujita, K. (2011). I acknowledge your help: Capuchin monkeys’ sensitivity to others’ labor. Animal Cognition, 14, 715–725.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takimoto, A., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2010). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) are sensitive to others’ reward: An experimental analysis of food-choice for conspecifics. Animal Cognition, 13(2), 249–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Talbot, C. F., Freeman, H. D., Williams, L. E., & Brosnan, S. F. (2011). Squirrel monkeys’ response to inequitable outcomes indicates a behavioural convergence within the primates. Biology Letters, 7(5), 680–682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, B. (2000). Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species. In F. Aureli & F. B. M. de Waal (Eds.), Natural conflict resolution (pp. 106–128). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobin, H., Logue, A., Chelonis, J. J., & Ackerman, K. T. (1996). Self-control in the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Animal Learning & Behavior, 24, 168–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Zimmermann, E., & Ross, M. D. (2011). Responding to inequities: Gorillas try to maintain their competitive advantage during play fights. Biology Letters, 7(1), 39–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Noordwijk, M. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (1985). Male migration and rank acquisition in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Animal Behaviour, 33(3), 849–861.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Noordwijk, M. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Intersexual food transfer among orangutans: Do females test males for coercive tendency? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(6), 883–890.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. (2004). Among orangutans: Red apes and the rise of human culture. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Wolkenten, M., Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Inequity responses of monkeys modified by effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(47), 18854.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallis, J. (1992). Chimpanzee genital swelling and its role in the pattern of sociosexual behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 28(2), 101–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (1997). Agonistic interventions in wild mountain gorilla groups. Behaviour, 134, 23–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werdenich, D., & Huber, L. (2002). Social factors determine cooperation in marmosets. Animal Behaviour, 64(5), 771–781.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, F. J. (1994). Food sharing in wild pygmy chimpanzees, Pan paniscus. Current Primatology, 2, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widdig, A., Nürnberg, P., Krawczak, M., Streich, W. J., & Bercovitch, F. B. (2001). Paternal relatedness and age proximity regulate social relationships among adult female rhesus macaques. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13769–13773. doi:10.1073/pnas.241210198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolovich, C. K., Evans, S., & French, J. A. (2008). Dads do not pay for sex but do buy the milk: Food sharing and reproduction in owl monkeys (Aotus spp.). Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 1155–1163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolovich, C. K., Feged, A., Evans, S., & Green, S. (2006). Social patterns of food sharing in monogamous owl monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 68(7), 663–674.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolovich, C. K., Perea-Rodriguez, J. P., & Fernandez-Duque, E. (2008). Food transfers to young and mates in wild owl monkeys (Aotus azarai). American Journal of Primatology, 70(3), 211–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. C. (1984). Biparental care in Aotus trivirgatus and Callicebus moloch. In M. F. Small (Ed.), Female primates: Studies by women primatologists (pp. 59–75). New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. C. (1994). The behavior and ecology of the owl monkey. In J. F. Baer, R. E. Weller, & I. Kakoma (Eds.), The owl monkey (pp. 97–112). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., Horita, Y., Takagishi, H., Shinada, M., Tanida, S., & Cook, K. S. (2009). The private rejection of unfair offers and emotional commitment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(28), 11520–11523.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, S., & Takimoto, A. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

Download references

Acknowledgments

S.F.B. was funded by a National Science Foundation Human and Social Dynamics grant (NSF SES 0729244) and a National Science Foundation CAREER award (NSF SES 0847351). We thank Lydia Hopper, Michael Beran, and Friederike Range for very helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript, as well as the researchers at Georgia State University’s Language Research Center and the Michale E. Keeling Center for Comparative Medicine and Research, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center for inspiring conversation about these ideas.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sara A. Price.

Appendix

Appendix

Chimpanzees

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are great apes, and one of our two closest living relatives (along with bonobos, Pan paniscus). Chimpanzees are highly social, living in large fission–fusion groups with complex social relationships and strong dominance hierarchies (de Waal, 1989; Harcourt & Waal, 1992). Males are philopatric and remain in their natal social communities, forming strong social bonds, while females emigrate when they reach adolescence. Chimpanzees also cooperate, for instance in territorial patrols (Boesch & Boesch-Achermann, 2000) or when hunting in groups (Boesch, 1994; Mitani & Watts, 2001). Although food sharing is in general rare outside of mothers and offspring (Goodall, 1986; Silk, 1978; although see Pruetz & Lindshield, 2011), meat sharing occurs (Goodall, 1986; Nishida, Hasegawa, Hayaki, Takahata, & Uehara, 1992), often in return for agonistic support (Mitani & Watts, 2001) or mating opportunities (Gomes & Boesch, 2009). Males may also share hard-to-get cultivated fruits with females (Hockings et al., 2007). The wide range of tolerant, cooperative behavior in the wild suggests that chimpanzees are aware of each other’s behaviors and the distribution of payoffs received through these interactions.

Chimpanzees also show a number of behaviors indicative that they pay attention to the equity of distributions. For instance, in laboratory cooperation studies, in which individuals must work together to acquire rewards, chimpanzees who were working for a single reward that must be shared were far more likely to succeed with a partner with whom they otherwise tolerantly share food (Melis et al., 2006b), and actively recruited the more tolerant partner (Melis et al., 2006a). Chimpanzees also were able to coordinate their responses in a Stag Hunt game such that both individuals achieved the highest reward, although this ability seemed to interact with experience, with chimpanzees with a more extensive history of testing outperforming those with less such experience (Brosnan, Parrish, et al., 2011).

Chimpanzees also respond to inequity in experimental studies (following the exchange procedure described in the main body of the text), although responses vary widely, based on factors that are not yet well understood. For instance, chimpanzees’ responses have varied between facilities, even when an identical protocol was used (Brosnan et al., 2005; Brosnan, Talbot, et al., 2010). Within the same facility, responses have varied based on the quality of individuals’ relationships (Brosnan et al., 2005), sex, and rank (Brosnan, Talbot, et al., 2010), with male and dominant chimpanzees responding more negatively to inequity than females and subordinates (when tested in same-sex pairs). These results seem to fit chimpanzee socio-ecology; males form much closer bonds, through coalitions and alliances often associated with hunting and patrolling, and therefore may be more sensitive to differing outcomes, while females spend most of their time foraging alone or with offspring, and so may be more focused on individual expectations. In addition, high-ranking individuals may be more accustomed to receiving a better share of a distribution or a better quality food resource. However, what is important to remember is that this is not always consistent across sites and protocols; sometimes sex and rank differences are seen, but other times they are not (Bräuer et al., 2006, 2009; Brosnan, Talbot, et al., 2010; Brosnan et al., 2005). In addition, even the orientation of the subjects to each other in the experimental set-up appears to make a difference in their responses (Brosnan, Talbot, et al., 2010). Thus, future study is needed to understand how this variation affects responses, and what this tells us about the evolution of inequity responses.

Bonobos

Bonobos (Pan paniscus) differ from chimpanzees in their social structure. Bonobo society is more female-centered, female-dominant, and egalitarian (de Waal, 1995). Males are philopatric and remain in their natal group, while females migrate around adolescence (Idani, 1991; Kano, 1982). Female bonobos have much stronger bonds compared to males, and bonobo females can cooperatively dominate males (Kano, 1992; Parish, 1994, 1996). Bonobos have been observed in the wild and captivity sharing both plants and meat (de Waal, 1992; Hohmann & Fruth, 1993; White, 1994), and may exchange food for mating access (Kuroda, 1984), although recent evidence indicates that chimpanzees may actually be more tolerant sharers (Jaeggi, Stevens, & Van Schaik, 2010). Bonobos have also cooperated in captive experiments, and in such studies were more successful than chimpanzees when food sources were monopolizable (Hare, Melis, Woods, Hastings, & Wrangham, 2007). Only one study has tested the inequity response in bonobos, and although individuals refused rewards twice as often when their partner got a better reward than when they got the same, lower-value reward, the result was not statistically significant, possibly due to the small sample size (Bräuer et al., 2009).

Gorillas

Gorillas (genus Gorilla) live in harems comprised of one adult male (silverback), multiple adult females with their offspring, and sometimes other males (Harcourt, 1978; Robbins, 1995). The silverback has strong bonds with his females (Harcourt, 1979a) but bonds between females vary, ranging from friendly to aggressive interactions (Harcourt, 1979b). In the wild, female gorillas have been observed cooperating and forming alliances in competition over food resources and to protect relatives from harm (Harcourt & Stewart, 2007; Watts, 1997). Although food sharing is rarely reported in the wild, gorillas have demonstrated food sharing in captivity (Maestripieri, Ross, & Megna, 2002; Schaller, 1963). Unfortunately, there is as yet very little captive research on social cognition in gorillas, although one recent study investigating responses to inequities during play fighting (Van Leeuwen et al., 2011) found that individuals worked to maintain inequities in their favor (e.g., running away after hitting, apparently so that the competitive advantage could be maintained). Future study on gorillas is needed to complete our understanding of the great apes.

Orangutans

Orangutans (Genus Pongo) are great apes that, in the wild, have a more solitary lifestyle compared to the other great apes. Although home ranges overlap (Galdikas, 1988), orangutans other than mother–offspring dyads spend most of their time alone (MacKinnon, 1974). Nonetheless, groups of orangutans may travel together, or feed together when fruit is abundant, and in captivity, orangutans show increased social behavior (Edwards & Snowdon, 1980). Based on research in the wild, food sharing among orangutans is rare (Bard, 1992; Jaeggi, van Noordwijk, & van Schaik, 2008; van Noordwijk & van Schaik, 2009). Also, unlike other apes in the wild, orangutans form coalitions and alliances to a lesser degree (van Schaik, 2004). However, the lack of cooperative behavior may again be due to limited opportunities, not limited abilities. In captivity, orangutans have both solved cooperative tasks to achieve food (Chalmeau, Lardeux, Brandibas, & Gallo, 1997) and traded tokens reciprocally to receive mutual benefits (Dufour, Pelé, Neumann, Thierry, & Call, 2009). Despite this, however, orangutans did not show negative responses to inequity (Bräuer et al., 2009; Brosnan, Flemming, et al., 2011).

Macaques

Macaques (genus Macaca) are Old World monkeys, and include two species that have received attention in inequity studies, rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Macaques are social, living in large, multi-male, multi-female groups, but tend to be despotic, with low social tolerance, and frequently use aggression to establish and reinforce social position in the asymmetrical dominance hierarchy (Smuts, Cheney, Seyfarth, Wrangham, & Struhsaker, 1987; Thierry, 2000). Although macaques are not known to cooperatively share food, or to successfully cooperate in experimental studies (Petit, Desportes, & Thierry, 1992), they demonstrate other forms of cooperative behavior in the wild. Long-tailed macaques form alliances in aggressive contexts (de Waal, 1977; van Noordwijk & van Schaik, 1985), reciprocate grooming to decrease aggression (Gumert & Ho, 2008), and exchange grooming for access to sexually active females, infant handling, and support (Gumert, 2007a, 2007b; Massen, 2010). Female rhesus macaques show cooperative behavior (grooming and agonistic aid) toward kin (Widdig, Nürnberg, Krawczak, Streich, & Bercovitch, 2001). Possibly connected to this, long-tailed macaques have shown prosocial behavior in the laboratory, choosing options that bring food to their partners (Massen, Van Den Berg, Spruijt, & Sterck, 2010). However, in most cases prosocial behavior was limited to female kin pairs or the dominant member of the pair, the latter of which may have been an attempt to curry favor with the subordinate. In fact, rank predicted prosocial sharing better than did relationship quality (Massen, Luyten, Spruijt, & Sterck, 2011). Related to this, dominant long-tailed macaques showed responses to inequity when the level of effort required was small (Massen, van den Berg, et al., 2011). As with long-tailed macaques, rhesus monkeys coordinated in a Stag Hunt game (Brosnan, Wilson, & Beran, 2012), and also have responded negatively to inequity, although only once they have reached ~2 years of age (Hopper et al., in review).

Capuchin Monkeys

Brown capuchins (genus Cebus) are New World monkeys that live in small groups comprised of related females with offspring and several males. Capuchin society is rather tolerant (Fragaszy et al., 2004); capuchins both share food (Perry & Rose, 1994) and cooperate, including cooperative hunting (Rose, 1997), defense against predators (Boinski, 1988; Rose, 1994), and coalitions against neighboring groups (Gros-Louis, Perry, & Manson, 2003). These coalitions are especially interesting because, unlike chimpanzee males, who form strong bonds and who often form coalitions for aggressive purposes, capuchin males are not philopatric and do not form close affiliative bonds (Perry, 1998).

Capuchins have been widely used in cooperative studies, which have confirmed that they are aware of the contingencies of cooperation, including the role of a partner, the effort required, and the distribution of outcomes (see Brosnan, 2010 for review). Capuchins have also shared food (de Waal, 1997), and after successful cooperative trials, the amount of food shared increased (de Waal & Berger, 2000). On the other hand, in the Stag Hunt game, capuchins showed the least structured coordination behavior compared to humans, rhesus, and chimpanzees, and apparently require cues to be able to coordinate (Brosnan, Parrish, et al., 2011; Brosnan et al., 2012). Capuchins have also demonstrated prosocial preferences, which seem to be affected by dominance rank or social closeness with the partner, whether or not they can see the partner, and reward distribution (de Waal, Leimgruber, & Greenberg, 2008; Takimoto et al., 2010). They have even brought their partner more food than they received (Brosnan, Houser, et al., 2010; Lakshminarayanan & Santos, 2008).

Several studies have confirmed a negative response by an individual when unequal distributions resulted in a partner receiving a better reward (Brosnan & de Waal, 2003; Brosnan, Houser, et al., 2010; Fletcher, 2008; Takimoto et al., 2010; Van Wolkenten et al., 2007).They have also been sensitive to the effort required to perform the task, at least in some contexts (Hattori et al., 2005, but see Fontenot et al., 2007; van Wolkenten et al., 2007). Finally, individuals who were working together for rewards were more sensitive to their partners’ behavior than to their outcomes, continuing to cooperate as long as both the partners received the superior reward sometimes (Brosnan et al., 2006). This latter study demonstrated that they could extrapolate rewards across multiple trials, presumably an essential ability given that cooperative encounters do not result in equal outcomes on every trial.

Squirrel Monkeys

Squirrel monkeys (genus Saimiri) share both a phylogenetic Family and a similar ecology with capuchins, but differ in their social structure and behavior (Boinski, 1999). Most importantly for this discussion, they demonstrate only limited cooperative behavior in the wild. Adult females with neonates will cooperate in anti-predator vigilance and defense (Boinski, 1987), and males form coalitions to immigrate into a new group (Boinski, 1999; Mitchell, 1994). Squirrel monkeys have not been tested experimentally for cooperative behavior, but did show reduced food sharing behavior, except when harassed by a partner (Stevens, 2004). Squirrel monkeys also did not respond negatively to inequity (Talbot et al., 2011).

Tamarins

Tamarins (genus Saguinus) are cooperatively breeding New World monkeys that live in groups in which, typically, only a single female is reproductively active (Sussman & Garber, 1987). Cooperation is vital in tamarin societies; data from the field and captivity have demonstrated that infant survival is directly related to number of caretakers present (Garber, Moya, & Malaga, 1984; Savage, Snowdon, Giraldo, & Soto, 1996; Snowdon, 1996). Food sharing is also common. Field studies show that food sharing is used to help the young locate and obtain prey that may be hard to acquire, and this food sharing decreases as the young become more competent (Rapaport, 2006; Rapaport & Ruiz-Miranda, 2002). Studies in captivity reinforce tamarins’ tendencies to share food (Cronin & Snowdon, 2008; Cronin, Kurian, & Snowdon, 2005; Feistner & Price, 1999, 2000; Price & Feistner, 2001; Rapaport, 1999). Data conflict on whether tamarins are prosocial (Cronin, Schroeder, Rothwell, Silk, & Snowdon, 2009; Cronin, Schroeder, & Snowdon, 2010; Stevens, 2010). Tamarins showed little evidence of an inequity response, although they were more likely to respond negatively to a lower-value reward when any effort was involved (Neiworth et al., 2009).

Marmosets

Marmosets (genus Callithrix) are also New World monkeys that are in many ways similar to tamarins (they share a phylogenetic family and are also cooperative breeders; Koenig, 1995; Sussman & Garber, 1987). Marmosets tend to share food in the wild (Brown, Almond, & Bergen, 2004; Goldizen, 1987) and in captivity (Brown, Almond, Bates, 2005). Group territorial defense is also common (Lazaro-Perea, 2001). In experimental studies, marmosets demonstrated cooperative behavior that depended on the distribution of roles in a cooperative task as well as the tolerance of high-ranking individuals (Werdenich & Huber, 2002) and prosocial behavior (Burkart, Fehr, Efferson, & Van Schaik, 2007). However, marmosets were not sensitive to situations in which a partner received a better reward (Freeman et al., in review).

Owl Monkeys

Owl monkeys (genus Aotus) are also New World monkeys that, while not considered cooperative breeders, are monogamous and rely on dual-parental care for the survival of offspring (Fernandez-Duque et al., 2001; Wright, 1994). Food sharing has been observed in the wild (Wolovich, Perea-Rodriguez, & Fernandez-Duque, 2008) and in captivity (Wolovich, Feged, Evans, & Green, 2006; Wolovich, Evans, & French, 2008). They also exhibit biparental care, a form of cooperation in which males help to transport and groom infants (Rotundo, Fernandez-Duque, & Gimenez, 2002; Rotundo, Fernandez-Duque, & Dixson, 2005; Wright, 1984). However, as with the cooperative breeders, owl monkeys did not respond negatively to inequity in an experimental context (Freeman et al., in review).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Price, S.A., Brosnan, S.F. To Each According to his Need? Variability in the Responses to Inequity in Non-Human Primates. Soc Just Res 25, 140–169 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-012-0153-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-012-0153-z

Keywords

Navigation