Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Understanding and Shifting Drug-Related Decisions: Contributions of Automatic Decision-Making Processes

  • Substance Use and Related Disorders (F Levin and E Dakwar, Section Editors)
  • Published:
Current Psychiatry Reports Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While substance use is common, only a minority of individuals who use drugs or alcohol develop problematic use. An understanding of the factors underlying the transition from substance use to misuse may improve prevention and intervention efforts. A key feature of substance misuse is ongoing decisions to use drugs or alcohol despite escalating negative consequences. Research findings highlight the importance of both relatively automatic, associative cognitive processes and relatively controlled, deliberative, and rational-analytic cognitive processes, for understanding situational decisions to use drugs. In this review, we discuss several cognitive component processes that may contribute to decision-making that promotes substance use and misuse, with a focus on more automatic processes. A growing body of evidence indicates that relative differences in the strength of these component processes can account for individual differences in the transition from substance use to misuse and may offer important avenues for developing novel intervention strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The reader is referred to a special issue of CNS Spectrums (2014, 40) that covers many of these points on attentional bias in greater depth.

  2. It should be noted that a methodological characteristic that varies across studies of impulsivity but was beyond the scope of this review to explore, is the tangibility of the task rewards (i.e., hypothetical or real). Burgeoning evidence indicates that the performance of substance users on impulsivity tasks, and the associated neural response differs along this axis (e.g., 79. Vadhan NP, Hart CL, Haney M, van Gorp WG, Foltin RW. Decision-making in long-term cocaine users: Effects of a cash monetary contingency on Gambling task performance. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2009;102(1–3):95–101. 80•. Hulka LM, Eisenegger C, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Bendrick K, et al. Altered social and nonsocial decision making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychological medicine. 2014;44(5):1015–28, 81. Chung T, Geier C, Luna B, Pajtek S, Terwilliger R, Thatcher D, et al. Enhancing response inhibition by incentive: comparison of adolescents with and without substance use disorder. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2011;115(1–2):43–50.). It remains to be seen whether ecological validity and clinical relevance also differ in a similar fashion.

References

Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as: • Of importance

  1. Hasin DS, Stinson FS, Ogburn E, Grant BF. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(7):830–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Compton WM, Thomas YF, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(5):566–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Anthony J, Warner L, Kessler R. Comparative Epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalants: basic findings from the National Comorbidity Survey. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1994;2:244–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Dutra L, Stathopoulou G, Basden SL, Leyro TM, Powers MB, Otto MW. A meta-analytic review of psychosocial interventions for substance use disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(2):179–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Edwards G. The alcohol dependence syndrome: a concept as stimulus to enquiry. Br J Addict. 1986;81(2):171–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Evans J, Stanovich K. Dual-Process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:223–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1458–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Garavan H, Weierstall K. The neurobiology of reward and cognitive control systems and their role in incentivizing health behavior. Prev Med. 2012;55(Suppl):S17–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Newell B. "Wait! Just let me not think about that for a minute": What role do implicit processes play in higher-level cognition? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2015;24:65–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Price C, Wells E, Donovan D, Rue T. Mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy as an adjunct to women's substance use disorder treatment: A pilot feasibility study. J Subst Abus Treat. 2012;43:94–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13(4):500–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Craig AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(2):189–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Paulus MP, Stewart JL. Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76 Pt B:342–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Paulus MP. Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry–altered homeostatic processing? Science. 2007;318(5850):602–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L, Dunn BD. The role of interoception in addiction: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(8):1857–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Paulus MP, Stewart JL, Haase L. Treatment approaches for interoceptive dysfunctions in drug addiction. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:137.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. May AC, Stewart JL, Migliorini R, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. Methamphetamine dependent individuals show attenuated brain response to pleasant interoceptive stimuli. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;131(3):238–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gu X, Liu X, Van Dam NT, Hof PR, Fan J. Cognition-emotion integration in the anterior insular cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(1):20–7.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Stewart JL, Parnass JM, May AC, Davenport PW, Paulus MP. Altered frontocingulate activation during aversive interoceptive processing in young adults transitioning to problem stimulant use. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:89.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Stewart JL, May AC, Poppa T, Davenport PW, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. You are the danger: attenuated insula response in methamphetamine users during aversive interoceptive decision-making. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;142:110–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The insula and drug addiction: an interoceptive view of pleasure, urges, and decision-making. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5–6):435–50.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Naqvi NH, Gaznick N, Tranel D, Bechara A. The insula: a critical neural substrate for craving and drug seeking under conflict and risk. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1316:53–70.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science. 2007;315(5811):531–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Reed SC, Haney M, Evans SM, Vadhan NP, Rubin E, Foltin RW. Cardiovascular and subjective effects of repeated smoked cocaine administration in experienced cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;102(1–3):102–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Foltin RW, Haney M. Intranasal cocaine in humans: acute tolerance, cardiovascular and subjective effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;78(1):93–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ramesh D, Haney M, Cooper ZD. Marijuana's dose-dependent effects in daily marijuana smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21(4):287–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Bedi G, Phan KL, Angstadt M, de Wit H. Effects of MDMA on sociability and neural response to social threat and social reward. Psychopharmacology. 2009;207(1):73–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schulteis G. The role of interoception and alliesthesia in addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;94(1):1–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. De Houwer J. What are implicit measures and why are we using them? In: Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2006. p. 11–28.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  31. Vadhan NP, Myers CE, Benedict E, Rubin E, Foltin RW, Gluck MA. A decrement in probabilistic category learning in cocaine users after controlling for marijuana and alcohol use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22(1):65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Stacy AW, Wiers RW. Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:551–75.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Wiers RW, Stacy AW. Implicit cogntition and addiction: an introduction. In: R.W. WaAWS, editors. Handbook of Implicit Cogntion and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2006. p. 1–8.

  34. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RC, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(2):263–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Roefs A, Huijding J, Smulders FT, MacLeod CM, de Jong PJ, Wiers RW, et al. Implicit measures of association in psychopathology research. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(1):149–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Pothos EM. The addiction-stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(3):443–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rooke SE, Hine DW, Thorsteinsson EB. Implicit cognition and substance use: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav. 2008;33(10):1314–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Wiers RW, de Jong PJ, Havermans R, Jelicic M. How to change implicit drug use-related cognitions in prevention: a transdisciplinary integration of findings from experimental psychopathology, social cognition, memory, and experimental learning psychology. Subst Use Misuse. 2004;39(10–12):1625–84. Provides a critical discussion of dual-process models for addiction and reviews several intervention strategies.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Intriligator JM, Klinger E. Attentional bias modification for addictive behaviors: clinical implications. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(03):215–24. Reviews attention control training for attentional bias, its effectiveness in reducing alcohol-related attentional bias and drinking behavior, and methods to export the training outside the clinic via mobile technology.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Luijten M, Field M, Franken IHA. Pharmacological interventions to modulate attentional bias in addiction. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(03):239–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;97(1–2):1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Carpenter KM, Schreiber E, Church S, McDowell D. Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample. Addict Behav. 2006;31(1):174–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Coskunpinar A, Cyders MA. Impulsivity and substance-related attentional bias: a meta-analytic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133(1):1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Smith DG, Ersche KD. Using a drug-word Stroop task to differentiate recreational from dependent drug use. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(03):247–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hester R, Luijten M. Neural correlates of attentional bias in addiction. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(3):231–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Carpenter KM, Martinez D, Vadhan NP, Barnes-Holmes D, Nunes EV. Measures of attentional bias and relational responding are associated with behavioral treatment outcome for cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38(2):146–54.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Fadardi JS, Cox WM. Reversing the sequence: Reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;101(3):137–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Wiers RW, Cox WM, Field M, Fadardi JS, Palfai TP, Schoenmakers T, et al. The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30(2):320–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Schoenmakers T, Wiers RW, Jones BT, Bruce G, Jansen ATM. Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction. 2007;102(3):399–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(3):260–5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Watson P, de Wit S, Hommel B, Wiers RW. Motivational Mechanisms and Outcome Expectancies Underlying the Approach Bias toward Addictive Substances. Front Psychol. 2012;3:440.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine's role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):391–431.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Zhou Y, Li X, Zhang M, Zhang F, Zhu C, Shen M. Behavioural approach tendencies to heroin-related stimuli in abstinent heroin abusers. Psychopharmacology. 2012;221(1):171–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Mogg K, Field M, Bradley BP. Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: an investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180(2):333–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Field M, Kiernan A, Eastwood B, Child R. Rapid approach responses to alcohol cues in heavy drinkers. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2008;39(3):209–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Field M, Eastwood B, Bradley BP, Mogg K. Selective processing of cannabis cues in regular cannabis users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;85(1):75–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cousijn J, Goudriaan AE, Ridderinkhof KR, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Wiers RW. Approach-bias predicts development of cannabis problem severity in heavy cannabis users: results from a prospective FMRI study. PLoS One. 2012;7(9):e42394.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306(5695):443–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Wiers RW, Gladwin TE, Hofmann W, Salemink E, Ridderinkhof KR. Cognitive bias modification and cognitive control training in addiction and related psychopathology: Mechanisms, clinical perspectives, and ways forward. Clin Psychol Sci. 2013;1(2):192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Sharbanee JM, Stritzke WG, Wiers RW, MacLeod C. Alcohol-related biases in selective attention and action tendency make distinct contributions to dysregulated drinking behaviour. Addiction. 2013;108(10):1758–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Peeters M, Wiers RW, Monshouwer K, van de Schoot R, Janssen T, Vollebergh WA. Automatic processes in at-risk adolescents: the role of alcohol-approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. Addiction. 2012;107(11):1939–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Wiers RW, Rinck M, Dictus M, van den Wildenberg E. Relatively strong automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-allele. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8(1):101–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Wiers CE, Stelzel C, Park SQ, Gawron CK, Ludwig VU, Gutwinski S, et al. Neural correlates of alcohol-approach bias in alcohol addiction: the spirit is willing but the flesh is weak for spirits. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;39(3):688–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Pieters S, Burk WJ, Van der Vorst H, Wiers RW, Engels RC. The moderating role of working memory capacity and alcohol-specific rule-setting on the relation between approach tendencies and alcohol use in young adolescents. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36(5):915–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. van Hemel-Ruiter ME, de Jong PJ, Wiers RW. Appetitive and regulatory processes in young adolescent drinkers. Addict Behav. 2011;36(1–2):18–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Sharbanee JM, Stritzke WG, Wiers RW, Young P, Rinck M, MacLeod C. The interaction of approach-alcohol action tendencies, working memory capacity, and current task goals predicts the inability to regulate drinking behavior. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(3):649–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Cousijn J, Snoek RW, Wiers RW. Cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance action tendencies: a field study in the Amsterdam coffee shops. Psychopharmacology. 2013;229(1):167–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Wiers RW, Rinck M, Kordts R, Houben K, Strack F. Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction. 2010;105(2):279–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Eberl C, Wiers RW, Pawelczack S, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;4:38–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Wiers RW, Eberl C, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients' approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(4):490–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl. 2002;8(2):75–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol. 2009;14(1):22–31.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Stevens L, Verdejo-García A, Goudriaan AE, Roeyers H, Dom G, Vanderplasschen W. Impulsivity as a vulnerability factor for poor addiction treatment outcomes: A review of neurocognitive findings among individuals with substance use disorders. J Subst Abus Treat. 2014;47(1):58–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Jentsch JD, Pennington ZT. Reward, interrupted: Inhibitory control and its relevance to addictions. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76 Part B:479–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Koffarnus MN, Gatchalian KM. Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: Emerging evidence. Pharmacol Ther. 2012;134(3):287–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Mukherjee D, Kable JW. Value-Based Decision Making in Mental Illness: A Meta-Analysis. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2(6):767–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Vadhan NP, Hart CL, Haney M, van Gorp WG, Foltin RW. Decision-making in long-term cocaine users: Effects of a cash monetary contingency on Gambling task performance. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;102(1–3):95–101.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Hulka LM, Eisenegger C, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Bendrick K, et al. Altered social and non-social decision-making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychol Med. 2014;44(5):1015–28. This study found that dependent, but not recreational cocaine users, exhibited risky decision-making on nonsocial tasks relative to controls.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Chung T, Geier C, Luna B, Pajtek S, Terwilliger R, Thatcher D, et al. Enhancing response inhibition by incentive: comparison of adolescents with and without substance use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;115(1–2):43–50.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;66(3):265–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Heil SH, Johnson MW, Higgins ST, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addict Behav. 2006;31(7):1290–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Fernández-Serrano M, Perales J, Moreno-López L, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. Neuropsychological profiling of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219(2):673–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Gorodetzky H, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Ersche KD. Differences in self-reported decision-making styles in stimulant-dependent and opiate-dependent individuals. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186:437–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Coskunpinar A, Dir AL, Cyders MA. Multidimensionality in Impulsivity and Alcohol Use: A Meta-Analysis Using the UPPS Model of Impulsivity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(9):1441–50.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Dick DM, Smith G, Olausson P, Mitchell SH, Leeman RF, O'Malley SS, et al. Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol. 2010;15(2):217–26.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Trifilieff P, Martinez D. Imaging addiction: D2 receptors and dopamine signaling in the striatum as biomarkers for impulsivity. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76 Part B:498–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Alfonso JP, Caracuel A, Delgado-Pastor LC, Verdejo-García A. Combined goal management training and mindfulness meditation improve executive functions and decision-making performance in abstinent polysubstance abusers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;117(1):78–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Washio Y, Higgins ST, Heil SH, McKerchar TL, Badger GJ, Skelly JM, et al. Delay discounting is associated with treatment response among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2011;19(3):243–8.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Bickel WK, Quisenberry AJ, Moody L, Wilson AG. Therapeutic Opportunities for Self-Control Repair in Addiction and Related Disorders: Change and the Limits of Change in Trans-Disease Processes. Clin Psychol Sci J Assoc Psychol Sci. 2015;3(1):140–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Bickel WK, Landes RD, Kurth-Nelson Z, Redish AD. A Quantitative Signature of Self-Control Repair: Rate-Dependent Effects of Successful Addiction Treatment. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2(6):685–95. Reviews the association between changes in discounting rates and treatment response across five treatment studies.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Gross J, Woelbert E, Zimmermann J, Okamoto-Barth S, Riedl A, Goebel R. Value signals in the prefrontal cortex predict individual preferences across reward categories. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2014;34(22):7580–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  95. Levy DJ, Thavikulwat AC, Glimcher PW. State dependent valuation: the effect of deprivation on risk preferences. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e53978.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Clithero JA, Rangel A. Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(9):1289–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Levy DJ, Glimcher PW. The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012;22(6):1027–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324(5927):646–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Goldstein RZ, Woicik PA, Moeller SJ, Telang F, Jayne M, Wong C, et al. Liking and wanting of drug and non-drug rewards in active cocaine users: the STRAP-R questionnaire. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24(2):257–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Compliance with Ethics Guidelines

Conflict of Interest

Kenneth M. Carpenter, Gillinder Bedi, and Nehal P. Vadhan declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kenneth M. Carpenter.

Additional information

This article is part of the Topical Collection on Substance Use and Related Disorders

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carpenter, K.M., Bedi, G. & Vadhan, N.P. Understanding and Shifting Drug-Related Decisions: Contributions of Automatic Decision-Making Processes. Curr Psychiatry Rep 17, 65 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0607-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0607-8

Keywords

Navigation