No consensus exists regarding the optimal depth of sedation for colonoscopy, with preferences among countries, regions, healthcare facilities, endoscopists, sedation providers, and patients ranging from no sedation at all to intentional general anesthesia. Patients often describe optimal sedation as amnesia for the procedure. A recent survey of patients awaiting colonoscopy indicated that awareness during the procedure was of greater concern than vomiting, prolonged drowsiness, respiratory complications, and even an incomplete colonoscopy examination.1 Other studies found that fear of intraprocedural pain and embarrassment were significant barriers to compliance with a colonoscopy examination.2,3 In contrast, healthcare professionals may prioritize safety, ease of colonoscopy completion, and rapid and complete cognitive recovery to facilitate early discharge. Arguably, both lighter and deeper sedation can potentially achieve these goals.

Drug-induced hypnosis occurs on a continuum from mild drowsiness to deep unconsciousness, with the threshold for loss of recall occurring at a lighter depth of sedation than the lack of response to command.4 Consequently, even ‘light’ sedation may result in procedural amnesia despite clinical sedation scores showing preserved responsiveness.5 It is possible, therefore, that the incidence of recall may be equivalent between light and deep sedation protocols.5,6 Nevertheless, studies are lacking that make a direct comparison between light and deep sedation (using the same drugs) for colonoscopy with an objective measure of sedation depth, a validated assessment of post-procedure cognitive impairment, and measurements of complications and patient satisfaction. A lack of direct comparisons such as this impair the ability to advise patients about the advantages and disadvantages of different sedation depths, including the likelihood of procedural recall.

We therefore investigated whether light (bispectral index [BIS] 70-80) and deep (BIS < 60) propofol and fentanyl-based sedation for adult patients presenting for elective outpatient colonoscopy were equivalent with respect to the primary outcome of procedural recall. Secondary outcomes examined included complications, patient and anesthesiologist satisfaction with sedation, endoscopist ease of completion, as well as patient recovery profiles and post-procedure cognitive impairment.

Methods

With Institutional Ethics Committee approval (Melbourne Health, Aug 2011; 2011.066), adult patients scheduled for elective outpatient colonoscopy under sedation administered by an anesthesiologist at the Royal Melbourne Hospital were screened for eligibility. Written informed consent was obtained from eligible patients. Inclusion criteria were patients who were 18 years of age or older and American Society of Anesthesiologists’ (ASA) physical status I-III. Exclusion criteria were patients undergoing an emergency or inpatient colonoscopy or other endoscopic procedures in addition to colonoscopy as well as those with cognitive, psychiatric, or language barriers to obtaining consent.

The primary outcome of recall was assessed in the post anesthesia care unit (PACU) using the modified Brice questionnaire.7,8 The questionnaire was administered in the PACU once the patient had returned to full alertness (i.e., a score of 5 on the Modified Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation [MOAA/S])9 following sedation. Patient satisfaction with sedation was scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = very dissatisfied; 5 = very satisfied). The ease of colonoscopy completion was rated by the endoscopist on an 11-point Likert scale (0 = very difficult; 10 = very easy).

Cognitive testing was undertaken with the Cogstate brief test battery10-14 using a computerized playing card interface for the following tasks:

  1. 1.

    Detection (“Has the card turned over?”) with speed of response as the outcome

  2. 2.

    Identification (“Is the card red?”) with speed of response as the outcome

  3. 3.

    One-card learning (“Have you seen this card before in this task?”) with accuracy as the outcome

  4. 4.

    One-back memory (“Is the card the same as the previous card?”) with both speed and accuracy as outcomes.

Baseline cognitive testing occurred on the day of the procedure after admission to the endoscopy unit. Post-procedure testing occurred once the patient was assessed by unit nursing staff as meeting hospital discharge criteria. Post-procedure impairment in cognitive function was defined by deterioration in performance from baseline, with each task considered separately.

For our analysis, we defined a number of possible complications a priori along with minor and serious subcategories.15 Minor hypoxia was determined if the oxygen saturation was < 90% and the patient was unresponsive to > 15 sec of jaw thrust, verbal stimulus, or increased oxygen flow. Serious hypoxia was considered if bag-mask ventilation or intubation was required. Minor airway obstruction was diagnosed if noisy breathing or an obstructed respiratory pattern occurred requiring jaw thrust or chin lift. Serious airway obstruction was considered if insertion of an oral or nasopharyngeal airway was required. Minor hypotension was defined as a 20% decrease in mean blood pressure from pre-procedure baseline, systolic blood pressure < 90 mmHg, and/or diastolic blood pressure < 50 mmHg. Serious hypotension was considered if vasoactive drugs or a fluid bolus was required. Minor bradycardia was defined as a 25% decrease in heart rate from pre-procedure baseline or a heart rate < 55 beats·min−1. Serious bradycardia was considered if chronotropic drugs were required or if cardiac arrest occurred. Lastly, minor agitation was defined as patient movement during the procedure that required physical restraint or medication bolus. Serious agitation was considered if it required either interruption or abandonment of the procedure or patient injury.

Two hundred patients were randomized by a computer-generated sequence (www.randomization.com) to receive light (BIS 70-80) or deep (BIS < 60) sedation. Randomization results were concealed in opaque envelopes until after consent was obtained and baseline testing was completed. Patients and study personnel undertaking post-procedure observations were blinded to group allocation. The attending anesthesiologists applied standard monitoring plus a BIS sensor and recorded baseline patient data. Sedation was then commenced with a fentanyl bolus (age < 70 yr = fentanyl 0.7 μg·kg−1 up to maximum of 100 μg; age ≥ 70 yr = fentanyl 0.5 μg·kg−1 up to maximum of 75 μg) and an effect-site-guided target-controlled propofol infusion using the Schnider model.16 Once the BIS target was achieved, the procedure commenced and the propofol target concentration was adjusted by the anesthesiologist as required to maintain the BIS within range.

The defined complications during sedation were managed as clinically appropriate and recorded by the anesthesiologist. At the end of the procedure, the endoscopist rated the ease of completion of the colonoscopy on an 11-point Likert scale, and both the endoscopist and anesthesiologist rated satisfaction with sedation on a five-point Likert scale.

In the PACU, routine clinical care was undertaken, and when the patients were alert (MOAA/S = 5), they were assessed for procedural recall. Once the patients were ready for hospital discharge according to the Chung discharge criteria,17 cognitive testing was repeated and patient satisfaction with sedation was recorded.

Statistical analysis

The incidence of recall during propofol sedation for colonoscopy has been reported to be 3-4% when a deeper sedation was targeted.18-20 Therefore, no recall in 95% of patients undergoing deep propofol-based sedation was used as the baseline for sample size calculation. An equivalence margin of 10% in the incidence of recall between the light and deep sedation groups was selected by investigator consensus, given the lack of literature to inform this decision. For a significance level of 0.05 and a power of 0.8, each group therefore needed 76 participants (total = 152) to show equivalence. A recruitment target of 200 patients was set to account for dropouts and to allow sufficient power for analysis of secondary outcomes.

Data were analyzed on an intention-to-treat basis, except for recall, which was also analyzed on an as-per-protocol basis given the equivalence hypothesis for the primary endpoint.

The BIS data were collected electronically at one-minute intervals on a stand-alone BIS monitor, and the output was downloaded into an Excel spreadsheet. If the signal quality index was < 50, the data at that time point were excluded. All remaining values from commencement of drug administration to completion of the procedure were included.

Continuous data were tested for normality and then summarized using mean (SD) if normally distributed or median (interquartile range [IQR]) if non-normally distributed. Effect sizes and confidence intervals (CIs) are provided, and CIs for the non-normally distributed data were calculated by the Somers’ D Z transformation method. For each Cogstate task, non-normally distributed data were arcsine or log-transformed to permit the use of parametric methods before calculating the mean (SD) of speed of response and accuracy. Categorical data were summarized using number (%).

The difference in the rate of recall between patients in the light and deep sedation groups was analyzed for equivalence using the two one-sided test (TOST) approach and assessment of the equivalence margin.21 With this method, equivalence would be established if the 90% CI for the difference between recall rates with light and deep sedation fell within the prespecified 10% equivalence margin.

Comparisons between the two randomized groups were made using unpaired two-tailed Student’s t tests (normally distributed data), Wilcoxon rank-sum tests (non-normally distributed data), and Chi square or Fisher’s exact tests (categorical data). Planned analyses included comparison of achieved BIS values, medication dosages, complications, durations, procedural ease, and MOAA/S scores in the PACU. Chi square testing was used to explore the relationship between ASA physical status and the occurrence of complications for the whole cohort and for the light and deep sedation groups. Comparisons between the discharge and baseline cognitive tests were made using paired two-tailed Student’s t tests. Analysis of covariance was used to analyze the effect of sedation depth, ASA physical status, and sex on post-procedure cognitive function, with pre-procedure cognition also included in the model. The effect of ASA physical status on cognitive recovery was analyzed by analysis of variance. Effect-size measures were used to express the magnitude of differences between (Cohen’s d) or within (Dunlap’s d) groups for the cognitive testing. For Cohen’s d and Dunlap’s d, generally an effect-size value > 0.1 = trivial effect; 0.1-0.3 = small effect; 0.3-0.5 = medium effect; and > 0.5 = large effect.22 A reliable change index (RCI) for each patient for the detection task in the Cogstate test battery was calculated to quantify clinically significant cognitive decline. The RCI allows for assessment of impairment from baseline testing at the individual patient level14 and has been used to assess post-sedation recovery.20 It is calculated by obtaining the task speed at discharge, subtracting the task speed at baseline, and dividing by the within-subject SD. An index > 1 has been calculated as impairment equivalent to a blood alcohol concentration of 0.05%.13,14 The Kruskal-Wallis rank test was used to compare sedation satisfaction between the two patient groups, the anesthesiologists, and the endoscopists. All recorded P values are two-sided. The majority of the analyses were conducted using STATA® version 12 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA), with supplementary analysis conducted using SPSS® version 22 (Released 2013, IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Of the 300 eligible patients screened for enrolment, 200 were subsequently randomized. Reasons for failure to randomize eligible patients are outlined in Fig. 1. One patient randomized to the light sedation group was mistakenly treated according to the deep sedation group protocol. The group characteristics were well balanced at baseline (Table 1). The median [IQR] BIS during the colonoscopy in patients randomized to light sedation (target BIS 70-80) was 69 [65-74] and in patients randomized to deep sedation (target BIS < 60) was 53 [46-59] (P < 0.001) (Table 2 and Fig. 2). Patients in the light sedation group received a significantly lower median [IQR] dose of propofol than patients in the deep sedation group (240 [170-320] mg vs 326 [250-420] mg, respectively; P < 0.001).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study patient flowchart

Table 1 Patient characteristics
Table 2 Sedation, procedure, and recovery characteristics
Fig. 2
figure 2

Bispectral index (BIS) data for each patient. Trend lines for light sedation (BIS 70-80) and deep sedation (BIS < 60) group medians at each time point with capture at one-minute intervals

As per our plan for statistical analysis, both the intention-to-treat and the as-per-protocol analyses were performed on the primary outcome. In both instances, the incidence of recall was 12% in the light sedation group vs 1% in the deep sedation group. Using the TOST procedure, the risk difference for the per-protocol analysis and for the intention-to-treat analysis was 0.11 (90% CI, 0.06 to 0.17) and 0.11 (90% CI, 0.05 to 0.17), respectively. Therefore, for both strategies equivalence was refuted at the 0.05 significance level with an equivalence margin of 10%.

Sedation and procedure times and ease of procedural completion were similar between groups (Table 2). Nevertheless, patients randomized to deep sedation were significantly more likely to suffer any complication during sedation than patients randomized to light sedation (66% vs 47%, respectively; P = 0.008) (Table 2). Specifically, airway obstruction (28% vs 14%, respectively; P = 0.017) and hypotension (46% vs 24%, respectively; P = 0.001) occurred more frequently in patients randomized to deep sedation than in patients randomized to light sedation. In contrast, agitation was more common in patients randomized to light sedation than in patients randomized to deep sedation (22% vs 11%, respectively; P = 0.033).

There was a low incidence of serious complications and no significant difference between groups (17%, deep sedation group vs 14%, light sedation group; P = 0.697). There was no significant relationship between ASA physical status and the incidence of complications in the whole cohort (P = 0.532), the light sedation group (P = 0.702), or the deep sedation group (P = 0.816). Exploratory post hoc analysis in patients with any complication vs those without complications revealed that the median [IQR] BIS was significantly lower (62 [50-68] vs 66 [55-72], respectively; P = 0.010) and the median [IQR] dose of propofol was significantly higher (320 [220-400] mg vs 250 [180-320]mg, respectively; P < 0.001).

The satisfaction scores for the anesthesiologists, endoscopists, and patients were all similar regardless whether light or deep sedation was administered (Table 2). Nevertheless, anesthesiologists were significantly less likely than endoscopists or patients to be highly satisfied with light (compared with deep) sedation (53% vs 79% vs 97%, respectively; P < 0.001). In addition, patients reporting recall were less likely to be satisfied with their overall sedation than patients not reporting recall (10/13 [77%] vs 185/186 [99%], respectively; P < 0.001).

Early recovery was more rapid in patients randomized to light sedation than in those randomized to deep sedation: median [IQR] MOAA/S on arrival in the PACU (4 [3-4] vs 2 [0-3], respectively; P < 0.001); mean (SD) time taken to MOAA/S = 5 [3 (4) min vs 7 (4) min, respectively; P < 0.001] (Table 2). There were also significant differences between the light and deep sedation groups in median [IQR] PACU discharge times (18 [15-22] min vs 20 [17-25] min, respectively; P = 0.013) and median [IQR] hospital discharge times (65 [57-80] min vs 74 [63-86] min, respectively; P = 0.001) favouring the light sedation group, although the magnitudes of the respective differences (i.e., 2 min and 9 min) were small (Table 2).

Cognitive testing revealed significant post-procedure impairment in the entire study cohort compared with baseline. This was reflected in slower psychomotor function and attention (detection and identification tasks) and decreased accuracy of memory (one-card learning task) (Table 3). Nevertheless, there was no difference between the light and deep sedation groups in the magnitude of post-procedure impairment, taking into account pre-procedure performance and age (Table 4). When cognitive deterioration (RCI > 1) was assessed post-procedure at the individual patient level, 35 patients (18%) experienced a clinically significant change with no significant difference between those randomized to light and deep sedation (19% vs 16%, respectively; P = 0.554).

Table 3 Cognitive testing pre and post procedure
Table 4 Cognitive testing by sedation depth

Discussion

Procedural recall rates in adults having elective outpatient colonoscopy with standardized BIS-guided propofol and fentanyl sedation were not equivalent in patients receiving light vs deep sedation. Nevertheless, since our study was designed to test an equivalence hypothesis, no meaningful conclusions can be made regarding the direction of effect of the different rates of recall in the light vs the deep sedation groups. Light sedation was associated with more intraoperative agitation but resulted in fewer cardiorespiratory complications and faster recovery than deep sedation. Sedation depth was not a significant predictor of cognitive impairment at hospital discharge, but statistically significant cognitive impairment (albeit mild) was recorded in both groups.

The incidences of recall in our study were consistent with other studies using propofol-based sedation of various depths.20,23,24 In studies of deep propofol-based sedation, procedural recall rates of 3-4% were reported.20,23 In contrast, in studies involving light to moderate propofol-based sedation, procedural recall rates of 14-50% were reported.24 Adjuvant drugs varied amongst these studies, and none of the studies were randomized trials. In this novel study, we randomized patients undergoing colonoscopy to either light or deep sedation so post-procedural recall could be compared following propofol and fentanyl-based sedation to a standardized anesthetic depth using BIS.

Patient satisfaction with sedation was high in this and previous studies.20,23,24 Most patients who did experience recall were still satisfied with their sedation. Chatman et al. 1 found that pre-procedure discussion about awareness reduced patients’ anxiety regarding this possibility. Therefore, with appropriate discussion and consent, light sedation for colonoscopy may be acceptable to patients despite the higher incidence of recall. In another study, patients undergoing colonoscopy were randomized to either routine general anesthesia or patient-controlled propofol sedation. Results showed that significantly more patients would repeat the procedure with the same anesthetic if they received sedation.25 Further, 25% of patients did not use any sedation for the procedure,25 which suggests that deep sedation may not be desired by a large number of patients.

Anesthesiologists were equally satisfied with light or deep sedation, but they were less likely to be satisfied with the sedation than patients and endoscopists. The basis of this lower satisfaction may have been the requirement to administer propofol by target-controlled infusion rather than by intermittent bolus delivery. Bolus delivery is preferred by many sedation providers perhaps because titration is more difficult to perform. The target-controlled infusion of propofol was used as protocol to assist with maintaining a constant depth of sedation and to facilitate modelling of effect-site concentrations.

Light and deep sedation each have advantages and disadvantages that must be balanced against the incidence of recall. The logistic factors were either the same between groups (sedation time, procedure time, procedural ease) or favoured the light sedation group (time until MOAA/S = 5, time in PACU, and time until hospital discharge). Although the magnitudes of the differences were small, the procedural or financial ramifications may be significant given the large number of colonoscopies performed. The incidence of complications was either not different between groups (hypoxia, bradycardia) or dependent on the group. Agitation was more common in the light sedation group, while hypotension and airway obstruction were common in the deep sedation group. Progression to serious consequences was rare, however, and not different between groups. Given our limited sample size, the conclusions relating to complications for endoscopy sedation are guarded, but our findings are consistent with other studies.15,19,25-28

At hospital discharge, patients in this study were cognitively impaired with reduced speed of response and task accuracy, as measured by the Cogstate brief test battery, which is similar to previous studies.10-14 These results did not differ between patients with light vs deep sedation; consequently, while light sedation might confer logistic advantages, they cannot be extended to cognitive improvement at hospital discharge. Patients were not re-tested; thus, no conclusions can be made between the light and deep sedation groups regarding the speed or completeness of recovery to baseline cognition. This would be an interesting extension to this work to investigate in the future. Nevertheless, this novel study uses a validated assessment tool to determine cognitive impairment after standardized light and deep sedation.

This study has several limitations. The number of patients is too small to assess rare complications and does not include longer term cognitive testing or assessment of extended postoperative recall. The baseline cognitive testing was performed on the day of the colonoscopy when patients were fasting, had received bowel preparation solution, and may have been anxious. It is possible that these conditions may have adversely impacted on baseline performance and that post-procedure impairment was underestimated as a result. In addition, the sedation profile of ‘light’ and ‘deep’ throughout each case does not coincide with the common practice of providing deep sedation at the start of the case - to facilitate transit of the sigmoid colon - with gradual lightening as the endoscope is removed.

This study is important because it compares clinically relevant light and deep sedation for meaningful outcomes, including recall, recovery times, and complications, including intraprocedural complications and cognitive impairment at discharge. The sedative medications used in this study reflect mainstream practice for many sedation providers, and the broad inclusion criteria make the findings relevant to a general endoscopy population. The findings from this study can inform the discussion between sedation providers and their patients regarding the risks and benefits of endoscopy sedation.