Skip to main content
Log in

‘I Mean, the Queen’s Fierce and the King’s Not’: Gendered Embodiment in Children’s Drawings

  • Original Article
  • Published:
International Journal of Early Childhood Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gender differences in children’s artwork have been the subject of study for over 100 years. The focus of early research was quite narrow, honing in on issues such as children’s gendered subject preferences, or their ability to render spatial relationships or include detail in their artwork. This has led to some stereotypical conclusions about gender with regard to particular aspects of visual representation. This paper speaks back to some of these stereotypes by discussing fundamental principles of meaning-making through drawing, and how the content of children’s artworks should be viewed in relation to their form, and the processes children used as the artworks evolved. Using social-constructivism as a theoretical framework, the methodology involved interlocutor-child dialogic improvisations, on a one-to-one basis, as each child engaged in graphic and body-based action while talking about aspects of the artwork and the processes of its creation through a free-form type of narrative. Semiotics is used as an analytical framework to describe three girls’ drawings (aged 5–8 years) who were selectively sampled from a larger study that involved over 100 children in drawing ‘what the future might be like’. These girls’ graphic-narrative-embodied artefacts are discussed in relation to three key themes: spatial relations and meaning; allegory and fantasy; and metaphor, abstract reasoning and connotation. The findings are discussed in relation social-cultural factors that might influences boys’ and girls’ gendered identities and, in turn, the content, form and processes of their artistic creations.

Résumé

Les différences liées au genre dans la production artistique des enfants sont étudiées depuis plus de 100 ans. La portée des premières recherches était très étroite, se limitant à des questions comme les sujets préférés selon le sexe des enfants ou leur habileté à rendre les relations spatiales ou à inclure des détails dans leurs travaux artistiques. Ces études initiales ont entrainé des conclusions stéréotypées sur le genre particulièrement sur certains aspects de la représentation visuelle. Cette étude revoit certains de ces stéréotypes en discutant de principes fondamentaux de construction de sens à travers le dessin, du contenu des dessins d’enfants qui devrait plutôt être vu en fonction de la forme, et des processus que les enfants utilisent au cours de l’évolution de leurs travaux. Avec le constructivisme social comme cadre théorique, la méthodologie utilise des dialogues improvisés entre l’interlocuteur et l’enfant, individuellement, alors que chacun des enfants s’adonne à des travaux graphiques et à une action corporelle alors que l’on parle d’aspects du travail et des processus employés pour le créer, ceci dans un type de narration libre. Un cadre analytique sémiotique est utilisé pour décrire les dessins de trois petites filles (âgées de 5 à 8 ans) sélectionnées d’une plus grande étude impliquant plus de 100 enfants qui dessinaient « ce à quoi l’avenir pourrait ressembler » . Les objets de narration graphique de ces petites filles sont discutées en fonction de trois thèmes clés: les relations spatiales et le sens; l’allégorie et l’imaginaire; la métaphore, le raisonnement abstrait et la connotation. Les résultats sont discutés par rapport aux facteurs socioculturels qui pourraient influencer l’identité de genre des garçons et des filles et, ensuite, le contenu, la forme et les processus employés dans leurs créations artistiques.

Resúmen

Las diferencias de género en el arte producido por niños/as ha sido sujeto de estudio por más de cien años. El enfoque de las primeras investigaciones fue bastante limitado, concentrándose en temas como las preferencias de temas en niños dependiendo del género, o su habilidad para reproducir relaciones espaciales o incluir elementos detallados en su arte. Esto ha arrojado algunas conclusiones estereotípicas sobre género con relación a aspectos específicos de la representación visual. Esta investigación contra argumenta algunos de aquellos estereotipos, discutiendo principios fundamentales de la creación de significado mediante el dibujo y la manera como el contenido de las obras de arte de niños debe verse en relación con su forma, así como los procesos utilizados por los niños en la medida en que sus obras evolucionan. Mediante el uso del constructivismo social como un marco teórico, la metodología incluyó improvisaciones de diálogo individual entre interlocutor y niño mientras el niño estaba ocupado realizando acciones basadas en trabajo gráfico y corporal y al mismo tiempo hablaba sobre aspectos de su obra y los procesos de su creación mediante una narración libre. Se utilizó la semiótica como marco analítico para describir tres dibujos de niñas (con edades que oscilan entre los 5 y los 8 años), quienes fueron escogidas selectivamente de un estudio más amplio que incluyó a más de cien niños que dibujaron el tema ‘Cómo será el futuro’. Los artefactos gráfico-narrativos de estas niñas se discuten sobre la base de tres temas principales: relaciones espaciales y significado; alegoría y fantasía; y metáfora, razonamiento abstracto y connotación. Los hallazgos se discuten con relación a factores socio-culturales que podrían influenciar las identidades de género de niños y niñas y, a su vez, el contenido, la forma y los procesos de sus creaciones artísticas.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A syntagm is a construction or sequence that functions as a relationship (i.e., linguistically, visually, spatially, temporally).

References

  • Anning, A., & Ring, K. (2004). Making sense of children’s drawings. Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Athey, C. (1990). Extending thought in young children: A parent–teacher partnership. London: Paul Chapman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, P. B. (1912). What London children like to draw. Journal of Experimental Pedagogy, 1, 185–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1957/1973). Mythologies. London: Fontana.

  • Barthes, R. (1977). Image–music–text. London: Fontana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, M. (2006). Drawing: The consequential progression of ideas. New Zealand Research in Early Childhood Education Journal, 9, 51–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, D. (2002). Semiotics: The basic. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duncum, P. (1997). Subjects and themes in children’s unsolicited drawings and gender socializations. In A. Kindler (Ed.), Child development in art (pp. 107–114). Reston, VA: National Arts Education Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, K. (1999). Children’s minds, talking rabbits and clockwork oranges: Essays on education. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flannery, K., & Watson, M. (1995). Sex differences and gender-role differences in children’s drawings. Studies in Art Education, 36(2), 114–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golomb, C. (2004). The child’s creation of a pictorial world (2nd ed.). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, F. (1926). Measuring of intelligence by drawings. New York: World Book Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerschensteiner, G. (1905). Die Entwicklung der zeichnerischen Begabung (Development of drawing aptitude). Munich: Carl Greber.

  • Kress, G., & van Leeuwen, T. (1996). Reading images: The grammar of visual design. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G., & van Leeuwen, T. (1998). Front pages: (The critical) analysis of newspaper layout. In A. Bell & P. Garrett (Eds.), Approaches to media discourse (pp. 186–219). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2008). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lankshear, C., & Knobel, M. (2011). New literacies: Everyday practices and classroom learning. Maidenhead: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobman, C. (2010). Creating developmental moments: Teaching and learning as creative activities. In C. M. Connery, V. P. John-Steiner, & A. Marjanovic-Shane (Eds.), Vygotsky and creativity: A cultural-historical approach to play, meaning making and the arts (pp. 199–214). New York: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malin, H. (2013). Making meaningful: Intention in children’s art making. IJADE, 32(1), 6–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, J. (1999). Helping children to draw and paint in early childhood. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, F., & Holzman, L. (2014). Lev Vygotsky: Revolutionary scientist (Classic ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paley, V. (1990). The boy who would be a helicopter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, P. (1986). The lively audience: A study of children around the TV set. Sydney: Allyn & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, R. (2014). The private and public worlds of children’s spontaneous art. Studies in Art Education, 55(2), 143–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ring, K. (2006). Supporting young children drawing: Developing a role. International Journal of Education Through Art, 2(3), 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, L. H. (1981). Measuring intelligences with Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test. Psychological Bulletin, 89(3), 487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1995). Play as performance, rhetoric, and metaphor. Play and Culture, 2, 189–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuman, D. M. (1999a). Gender style as form and content: An examination of gender stereotypes in the subject preference of children’s drawing. Studies in Art Education, 41(1), 40–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuman, D. M. (1999b). Sing a song of sixpence: An examination of sex difference in the subject preference of children’s drawings. Visual Arts Research, 25(1), 51–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Manen, M. (2001). Researching lived experience. London, ON: Althouse Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vollrath, M. M. (2006). Thank heaven for little girls: Girls’ drawings of representations of self. Visual Culture & Gender, 1, 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Wilson, B. (2005). More lessons from the superheroes of J. C. Holz. Art Education, 58(6), 18–24, 33–34.

  • Wilson, B., & Wilson, M. (1982). Teaching children to draw. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2007a). Graphic-narrative play: Authoring through multiple texts and fluid structures. International Journal of Education and the Arts, 8(8), 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2007b). Young children’s meaning-making through drawing and ‘telling’: Analogies to filmic textual features. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 32(4), 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2010). Understanding creativity in early childhood: Meaning-making and children’s drawings. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2011). Meaning, mediation and mythology. In D. Faulkner & E. Coates (Eds.), The expressive nature of creativity in childhood: New psychological perspectives (pp. 157–176). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2012). Arts education as a collective experience. In S. Wright (Ed.), Children, meaning-making and the arts (2nd ed., pp. 194–225). Frenchs Forest: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (2014). The art of voice: The voice of art—understanding children’s graphic–narrative–enactive communication. In D. Machin (Ed.), Handbook of Visual Communication (pp. 517–537). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susan Wright.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wright, S. ‘I Mean, the Queen’s Fierce and the King’s Not’: Gendered Embodiment in Children’s Drawings. IJEC 46, 391–406 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13158-014-0124-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13158-014-0124-7

Keywords

Navigation