Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Talking About Tools: Did Early Pleistocene Hominins Have a Protolanguage?

  • Thematic Issue Article: Symbols, Signals, and the Archaeological Record II
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article addresses the question of whether early Pleistocene hominins are plausibly viewed as having possessed a protolanguage, that is, a communication system exemplifying some but not all of the distinctive features of fully modern human language. I argue that the answer is “yes,” mounting evidence from the early Pleistocene “lithics niche.” More specifically, I first describe a cognitive platform that I think would have been sufficient, given appropriate socio-ecological conditions, for the creation and retention of a protolanguage. Then, using archaeological evidence pertaining to hominin lithic behavior from the early Pleistocene, I attempt to make plausible the idea that each of these cognitive abilities were in fact in place by this point in hominin evolutionary history and also that the requisite socio-ecological conditions were satisfied.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This lumps together significant hominin diversity; most noticeably, it collapses Homo habilis and Homo erectus into a single category. However, a considerable amount of the evidence discussed in this article pertains not just to erectine behavior but to habiline behavior as well (or at least to the behavior of the very earliest erectines).

  2. The term “protolanguage” is sometimes used to refer to the “mother language” of all extant languages. As will become clear below, this is not how I intend the term here. Rather, I simply mean a communication system that exemplifies some, but not all, of the distinctive features of fully modern human language.

  3. See Apperly (2010) for an excellent overview of the many ways in which theory of mind can be limited.

  4. See Planer (2017) for a discussion of this issue.

  5. One might object that I am stacking the deck against a heuristics-based approach to problem-solving (construed broadly) by positing the above heuristic rather than some alternative one. It would of course be possible to develop some other heuristic that would fare better in this example, but it would be just as easy to then present an alternative example in which it breaks down.

    More generally, attempts to avoid mental models (because, e.g., they are too representationally “rich”) in favor of heuristics rapidly leads to the proliferation of such heuristics, a result which many in cognitive science see as seriously problematic.

  6. Many authors wish to reserve the term “imitation” for cases where an individual not only copies the behavior of some actor but also represents the goal of that actor. I use the term here in a more permissive way so that representation of the actor’s goal or intention may be involved but need not be. (For a discussion of these two different ways of thinking about imitation and a defense of the latter see Leighton et al. 2010).

  7. I will use “her” (etc.) to refer to the actor and “him” (etc.) to refer to the reactor in what follows.

  8. In time, dispositions to produce and interpret signals in accordance with certain patterns or regularities might become “genetically specified.”

  9. This idea is consistent with recent modeling work on the emergence of combinatorial and compositional signals. See, for example, Franke (2016).

  10. I thank Kim Sterelny for this point.

  11. See Butterfill and Apperly (2013) for a discussion.

  12. See Pope et al. (2006) and Gowlett (2011) for a discussion of symmetry properties.

  13. As Kim Sterelny points out to me, an additional argument for improved meta-psychological abilities relative to those of chimpanzees can also be mounted on the basis of evidence suggesting ambush hunting of large game over 1 mya (Kübler et al. 2015). As this does not directly concern early Pleistocene lithic behavior, however, I leave it aside here.

  14. Sometimes up to distances of 100 km (Petraglia et al. 2005).

  15. One might worry that these hominins could have been adept at constructing mental models of physical situations while being unable to construct models of intentional systems. While such a scenario cannot be ruled out, I think recent neuroimaging work makes it unlikely. That work shows that constructing models of intentional systems (qua intentional systems) recruits many of the same neural networks and circuits that are recruited when we construct models of non-intentional systems. (See Apperly 2010 for an excellent overview and discussion.)

  16. As Peter Hiscock has pointed out to me, while this might be the more likely scenario, substitution is not strictly required to avoid fatigue as the collaborators might have simply worked slowly. I agree this is possible; but even a slow pace would not have rendered them immune to serious injury (though it may have decreased its incidence).

    Ideally, this issue about whether the activity in question involved substitution can be decided empirically: were we to find flakes reflecting different skill levels and/or individual “style” within a single reduction sequence, that would be strong evidence indeed for substitution. To the best of my knowledge, such a comparison was not carried out by Petraglia et al., however.

  17. See Sterelny (2012) for a detailed development and discussion of such learning systems.

  18. The coordinated hunting of Columbus monkeys by chimpanzees (Boesch 1994, 2002) is a nice example of a behavior that seems like it must involve sophisticated communication, but apparently does not.

  19. Sterelny (2012) urges us to adopt this orientation to human evolution in general.

  20. Wrangham seeks to explain the transition from Homo habilis to Homo erectus this way: erectines show shortened guts, larger brains, and dental reduction, all of which point (says Wrangham) to a diet containing cooked meat. Moreover, he uses changes in erectine morphology to argue that these hominins had made the transition from sleeping in trees to sleeping on the ground. He thinks such a transition would have been most unlikely in the absence of controlled fire to keep predators at bay overnight.

  21. See Bunn (2006) and Bunn and Pickering (2010) for early dates for large-game hunting (they argue for such hunting at 1.7 mya).

References

  • Apperly I (2010) Mindreaders: the cognitive basis of “theory of mind.” Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C (1994) Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 48(3):653–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C (2002) Cooperative hunting roles among Tai chimpanzees. Hum Nat 13(1):27–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunn HT (2006) Meat made us human. In: Ungar PS (ed) Evolution of the human diet: the known, the unknown, and the unknowable. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 191–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn HT, Pickering TR (2010) Bovid mortality profiles in paleoecological context falsify hypotheses of endurance running–hunting and passive scavenging by early Pleistocene hominins. Quatern Res 74(3):395–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfill SA, Apperly IA (2013) How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind Lang 28(5):606–637

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen W, Michael J (2015) From two systems to a multi-systems architecture for mindreading. New Ideas Psychol 40:48–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Féblot-Augustins J (1997) La circulation des matières premières au Paléolithique, vol 1 and 2, ERAUL 75. Université de Liège, Liège

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro JV, Plummer TW, Pobiner BL et al (2013) Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS ONE 8(4):e62174

  • Franke M (2016) The evolution of compositionality in signaling games. J Logic Lang Inf 25(3–4):355–377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble CS, Gowlett JAJ, Dunbar R (2014) Thinking big: how the evolution of social life shaped the human mind. Thames & Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett JA (2011). Special Issue: innovation and the evolution of human behavior. The vital sense of proportion: transformation, golden section, and 1:2 preference in Acheulean Bifaces. PaleoAnthropology, 174:174–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock P (2004) Slippery and Billy: intention, selection and equifinality in lithic artefacts. Cambridge Archaeol J 14(01):71–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock P (2014) Learning in lithic landscapes: a reconsideration of the hominid “toolmaking” niche. Biol Theory 9:27–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition 109(2):224–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kübler S, Owenga P, Reynolds SC et al (2015) Animal movements in the Kenya Rift and evidence for the earliest ambush hunting by hominins. Sci Rep 5:14011

  • Kyara O (1999) Lithic raw materials and their implications on assemblage variation and hominid behavior during Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University

  • Leighton J, Bird G, Heyes C (2010) “Goals” are not an integral component of imitation. Cognition 114(3):423–435

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petraglia MD, Shipton C, Paddayya K (2005) Life and mind in the Acheulean. In: The hominid individual in context. Routledge, London, pp 197–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Planer RJ (2017) Protolanguage might have evolved before ostensive communication. Biol Theory 12:72–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pope M, Russel K, Watson K (2006) Biface form and structured behaviour in the Acheulean. Lithics 27:44–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Potts R (1991) Why the Oldowan? Plio-Pleistocene toolmaking and the transport of resources. J Anthropol Res 47(2):153–176

  • Reti JS (2013) Methods for determining differential behaviors in stone tool production and application to the Oldowan of Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania and Koobi Fora, Kenya. Dissertation, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick

  • Shipton C, Petraglia MD, Paddayya K (2009a) Inferring aspects of Acheulean sociality and cognition from lithic technology. In: Adams B, Blades B (eds) Lithic materials and palaeolithic societies. Wiley, Chichester, pp 219–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipton CBK, Petraglia MD, Paddayya K (2009b) Stone tool experiments and reduction methods at the Acheulean site of Isampur Quarry, India. Antiquity 83(321):769–785

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a hostile world. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2012) The evolved apprentice. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stout D (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: an ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Curr Anthropol 43(5):693–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (2003) Chimpanzees understand psychological states–the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends Cogn Sci 7(4):153–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twomey T (2013) The cognitive implications of controlled fire use by early humans. Cambridge Archaeol J 23(01):113–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uller C, Nichols S (2000) Goal attribution in chimpanzees. Cognition 76(2):B27–B34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2009) Catching fire: how cooking made us human. Basic Books, New York

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Peter Hiscock and Kim Sterelny for their thoughtful comments on this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ronald J. Planer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Planer, R.J. Talking About Tools: Did Early Pleistocene Hominins Have a Protolanguage?. Biol Theory 12, 211–221 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0279-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0279-1

Keywords

Navigation