Dimensions of adolescent motivation as measured by higher-order factors in the school motivation analysis test

https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-4405(89)90028-9Get rights and content

Abstract

Although previous analyses of higher-order motivational dimensions have suggested at least seven major dynamic traits among adults, corresponding analyses among adolescents have not yet been undertaken in any satisfactory manner. To this end, the present article reports the results of a higher-order factor analysis of the subscale intercorrelations for the School Motivation Analysis Test (SMAT) on an Australian sample of 277 Year 10 students enrolled in various senior high schools. The results suggest that at least six factors maximally account for the common variance in the SMAT. Tentative interpretations of the higher-order factors cbtained are made and discussed in terms of their applicability for school psychology.

References (19)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (5)

  • Overarching personality paradigm: A neo-Cattellian psychometric model

    2019, Personality and Individual Differences
    Citation Excerpt :

    In similar vein, Boyle (1987b) also factor analytically reduced the 12 abnormal trait dimensions measured in the CAQ (Krug, 1980) down to just six second-stratum factors which were tentatively labelled: Depressive Schizophrenia, Psychopathic Dominance, Psychotic Inadequacy, Paranoid Depression, Helpless Depression, and Anxious Depression, thereby enabling more efficient measurement of psychopathology (cf. Boyle, 1985b; Boyle & Comer, 1990). Cattell had incorporated objective T-data tests into the MAT and its downward extensions, the SMAT and CMAT (Boyle, Start, & Hall, 1988, 1989a, 1989b; Cattell, 1982c, 1985, 1992; Cattell & Child, 1975; Sweney, Anton, & Cattell, 1986). The higher-stratum factor structure of objective motivation tests (T-data measures avoid item transparency, thereby minimising response distortion) has been investigated in several factor analytic studies (e.g., Barton, Dielman, & Cattell, 1986; Boyle, 1985a, 1988c, 1989b; Boyle & Start, 1988, 1989a; Boyle et al., 1989a; Cattell, 1981, 1985, 1992; Cattell & Kline, 1977).

  • Comprehensive assessment of children's psychological needs: Development of the child Reiss Motivation Profile for ages four to eleven

    2015, Learning and Individual Differences
    Citation Excerpt :

    One line of research has examined the effects of external reward on intrinsic motivation and has suggested that indeed, things like tangible rewards might be detrimental to intrinsic motivation (Deci et al., 1999). An important line of research has focused on the identification of the range of activities that individuals find intrinsically rewarding, individual differences in the intensity of these motives, and their role in career guidance, and school counseling (Boyle, Start, & Hall, 1989; Froiland, 2011; Froiland & Oros, 2013; Reiss & Havercamp, 1998). A number of perspectives suggest that certain motives are common to everyone and deeply rooted in human nature.

  • Simplifying the cattellian psychometric model

    2008, The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Volume 1 - Personality Theories and Models
View full text