Neuroeconomics

Neuroeconomics

Decision making and the brain
2009, Pages 425-440
Neuroeconomics

Chapter 28 - The Computation and Comparison of Value in Goal-directed Choice

https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-374176-9.00028-2Get rights and content

Publisher Summary

Value-based decision-making is a complex process that requires, at the very least, the deployment of five basic types of computations. First, a representation of the decision problem needs to be constructed. This entails identifying potential courses of action (e.g., to pursue a prey item or not), as well internal states (e.g., the hunger level) and external states (e.g., the threat level). Second, a value needs to be assigned to the different actions under consideration. In order to make sound decisions, these values need to be aligned with the benefits associated with each action. Third, the valuations are compared in order to make a choice. Fourth, after implementing the decision, the brain needs to measure the desirability of the outcomes that follow. Finally, this feedback is used to update the other processes in order to improve the quality of future decisions. These are not rigid categories, but they provide a useful and reasonable decomposition of decision-making into its composite parts.

References (0)

Cited by (19)

  • Value computation in humans

    2022, Evolution and Human Behavior
  • Filling the gaps: Cognitive control as a critical lens for understanding mechanisms of value-based decision-making

    2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    Citation Excerpt :

    In a typical cognitive control experiment (e.g., a Stroop, Eriksen flanker, or Simon task; Friedman and Robbins, 2021), the best course of action is indicated unambiguously (e.g., the participant is instructed to name the stimulus color, and that color is easy to identify), but choosing that response requires engaging control processes to overcome a bias towards automatically responding in a different way (e.g., based on another salient feature of the stimulus). While these two sets of tasks involve similar sets of computations (Rangel, 2009; Ritz et al., 2021), they are used to address questions that are in large part non-overlapping (Fig. 1). Decision-making research typically gives greater focus to the process of integrating information about the stimuli to select a response (Fig. 1A), taking account of previous learning, long-run preferences (e.g., risk aversion), motivational state (e.g., current hunger level), and more (Campbell-Meiklejohn et al., 2016; Lempert and Phelps, 2016; Louie et al., 2011).

  • Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making

    2013, Acta Psychologica
    Citation Excerpt :

    Although several theories have been described under the term ‘evidence accumulation model’ (see for instance Busemeyer & Townsend, 1993; Ratcliff & Smith, 2004), this paper considers the attentional drift diffusion model (aDDM), as outlined by Krajbich et al. (2010). Earlier versions of the drift diffusion model (Ratcliff & Smith, 2004) were concerned with perceptual discrimination; the aDDM extended this modus operandi to decision making (see Rangel, 2009 for an overview of both models). The aDDM assumes that decision makers accumulate evidence in favor of an alternative when fixating it, and that the speed of accumulation is a function of the value of the alternative relative to other alternatives in the choice set, which is called the bias rate.

  • Is neuroaccounting waiting in the wings? An essay

    2012, Accounting, Organizations and Society
    Citation Excerpt :

    Also, evidence on how the brain actually perceives and evaluates rewards and losses, and how it directs the human animal to achieve the intended rewards and avoid losses, e.g. the dopamine studies of Schultz (2009H), is expected to help in fine-tuning utility theory including its major variant, Prospect Theory (Fox & Poldrack, 2009H). Similarly, neural-level evidence on how values translate to choices (Bossaerts et al., 2009H; Glimcher, 2009H; Rangel, 2009H) and how primates, both those close to humans, e.g., chimpanzees in Silk (2009H), and further from humans, e.g., capuchin monkeys in Brosnan (2009H) perceive inequities, is likely to help understand and model choices and decisions at the individual level. Lastly, the ability of neuroeconomics methods to measure a person’s well-being in a variety of ways has the potential to shift the focus of behavioral researchers from choice to welfare (Jamison, 2008).

  • Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex

    2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
    Citation Excerpt :

    For example, Behrens and colleagues found that the VMPFC encoded the expected value of the chosen option based on the subjects’ own experiences as well as on social advice [64]. On the basis of these findings, it has been suggested that the VMPFC represents a common valuation signal that underlies different types of decision as well as decisions about different types of goods [31,41,59,68]. A related account [69] suggests that, whereas the OFC is involved in encoding the value of specific rewards, the VMPFC plays a specific role in value-guided decision-making about which of several options to pursue by encoding the expected value of the chosen option [64,70,71].

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text