Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 419, 15 February 2022, 113684
Behavioural Brain Research

Review
Methodological aspects of studying the mechanisms of consciousness

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113684Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The relationship between the mind and brain remains an extremely important area of neuroscience research.

  • The existence of a number of neurological data have not yet led to the creation theory of the regulation of consciousness.

  • Neurobiological data related to the biological mechanisms of the regulation of consciousness was thus summarized.

Abstract

There are at least two approaches to the definition of consciousness. In the first case, certain aspects of consciousness, called qualia, are considered inaccessible for research from a third person and can only be described through subjective experience. This approach is inextricably linked with the so-called "hard problem of consciousness", that is, the question of why consciousness has qualia or how any physical changes in the environment can generate subjective experience. With this approach, some aspects of consciousness, by definition, cannot be explained on the basis of external observations and, therefore, are outside the scope of scientific research. In the second case, a priori constraints do not constrain the field of scientific investigation, and the best explanation of the experience in the first person is included as a possible subject of empirical research. Historically, in the study of cause-and-effect relationships in biology, it was customary to distinguish between proximate causation and ultimate causation existing in biological systems. Immediate causes are based on the immediate influencing factors [1]. Proximate causation has evolutionary explanations. When studying biological systems themselves, such an approach is undoubtedly justified, but it often seems insufficient when studying the interaction of consciousness and the brain [2], [3]. Current scientific communities proceed from the assumption that the physical substrate for the generation of consciousness is a neural network that unites various types of neurons located in various brain structures. Many neuroscientists attach a key role in this process to the cortical and thalamocortical neural networks. This question is directly related to experimental and clinical research in the field of disorder of consciousness. Progress in this area of medicine depends on advances in neuroscience in this area and is also a powerful source of empirical information. In this area of consciousness research, a large amount of experimental data has been accumulated, and in this review an attempt was made to generalize and systematize.

Introduction

Consciousness research has always faced serious difficulties. The very definition of consciousness is rather vague and includes many interdisciplinary aspects related to psychology and philosophy. There is no generally accepted definition of consciousness, but most modern researchers agree that consciousness in its simplest form is “a feeling or awareness of internal or external existence” [4], [5], [6]. Until about the 20th century, the concept of consciousness was defined as "inner life", the introspection of personal thoughts, imagination and will. Today, most scientists include in the concept of consciousness individual experience, cognition and perception. It is also generally accepted that there may be different levels or orders of consciousness, as well as different types of consciousness, but assessing the level of consciousness is a non-trivial task. A separate question is whether only humans possess consciousness, or at least some animals also possess consciousness.

The founder of the concept of neuro-darwinism, Gerald Edelman, believed that it is necessary to distinguish between primary consciousness and higher (superior) consciousness [7]. Primary consciousness is a multimodular structure that brings together various sources of information. In contrast to the primary, higher consciousness develops in parallel with the acquisition of language, and continuously structures the experience gained by the individual. While primary consciousness links memory to current perception, higher consciousness directs the synthesis between memory patterns distributed over time. Edelman's model implies that the integrative activities required for higher consciousness arise at the border between the thalamocortical system and other areas of the brain. It is an autonomous dynamic nucleus that cannot be identified either as the brain as a whole or as a specific part of the brain [7], [8].

Numerous studies, including with the use of modern imaging technologies, have been undertaken to answer the question "what happens in the brain when the awareness of the surrounding world disappears?" So, in a recent study, a change in the consciousness of an experiment was carried out with a simultaneous analysis of brain activity using positron emission tomography (PET, PET). Measurements of brain activity were carried out during wakefulness, under the influence of various anesthetics, as well as during non-rapid eye movements (non-REM) sleep [6]. The deep stage of general anesthesia, when the subsequent report on mental content did not contain any signs of awareness of the surrounding world, was considered as a confirmed lack of consciousness [6]. The level of consciousness of the test subjects was regulated by propofol or dexmedetomidine with a stepwise increase in the concentration of the drug [6].

Decades of research have led to a certain consensus based on the fact that consciousness, like numerous forms of complex behavior, are network-level phenomena, that is, a phenomenological product of a neural network [9], [10]. Highly organized social behavior requires high-level integration of various sensory inputs, synchronization of many motor patterns, and coordination of their activities within large-scale neural networks. Consequently, the accumulation of data on the functioning of neural networks contributes to the formation of a number of testable hypotheses regarding the neural foundations of cognitive functions, memory, and motivated behavior.

Currently, the study of consciousness consists of three main directions of levels: the search for neural correlates of consciousness, the mechanisms of functioning of these correlates and the reproducibility of the hypotheses put forward [11].

This manuscript is intended for neuroscientists, psychologists-clinicians, as well as other scientists involved in the field of relationship between consciousness and its brain substrate. Our aim was to more accurately formulate the main conclusions reached by experimental neurobiology while investigating the problem of the regulation of the mechanisms of consciousness by the human brain. It was used data from the last quarter of the previous century up to the present time.

Section snippets

Neural correlates of consciousness

Functional brain mapping is a critical area of neuroscience that has applications in both clinical and fundamental fields. In the latter case, functional mapping is directly related to the search for neural correlates of consciousness (NCC). In other words, this is a search for that hypothetical minimum set of neurons, without which the emergence of consciousness is impossible. On the other hand, even minimal changes in the functioning of the NCC should inevitably lead to a change in the state

Cortical and subcortical hypothesis

It can be assumed that modern neurobiology considers two subcortical structures - the thalamus and the claustrum, and two cortical systems as the main candidates for the role of NCC, and two cortical systems: the so-called "posterior cortical hot zone, PCHZ" - "posterior hot area" - a large area of the cortex at the junction of the parietal, the occipital and temporal lobes and the fronto-parietal cortical attention system - intraparietal sulcus (IPS) of the superior parietal lobe (SPL).

The

Claustrum as a candidate for the role of a key structure in the formation of consciousness

In addition to the thalamus, the claustrum (fence) is considered as a candidate for the role of the minimum necessary anatomical carrier of consciousness [71], [72]. Claustrum is a vertical curved plate of gray matter up to several millimeters thick (Fig. 1 C). The claustrum is part of the limbic system and is located lateral to the shell of the lenticular nucleus and medially under the insular cortex. Claustrum is found in the brain of all mammals [73].

A feature of claustrum is a large number

Epilepsy as a source of information about the state of consciousness

In clinical neurobiology, epilepsy provides a rich material for the study of consciousness, since an epileptic seizure and an epileptic aura are accompanied by a change or loss of consciousness [87]. At the same time, before and after the attack, the brain is in a normal state [88], [89]. The seizures that actually cause impaired consciousness are very diverse in terms of behavior and physiology. Historically, epileptic seizures are often associated with religious beliefs and are interpreted as

Von Economo neurons

Many researchers link regulation of consciousness with von Economo neurons, discovered and described in the early 20th century. Von Economo neurons (VENs) - spindle-shaped brain cells, named after the scientist who first described them, are found only in humans, apes, and some other highly social mammals. Apparently, these cells are present mainly in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and in one frontal islet (FI). Moreover, ACC is a rather unique structure of the brain in terms of the number

Astrocytic syncytium as a regulator of consciousness

In accordance with the "astrocentric" hypothesis, cortical and subcortical astrocytes, connected to each other through gap junctions, form numerous mosaic domains with a three-dimensional structure [104], [105]. Together with adjacent neurons, these domains are a matrix for expressing consciousness and binding sensory information to consciousness, as well as for storing memories [104].

With the help of gap junctions, astrocytes form astrocytic syncytium - a globally distributed network of

Coma and minimally conscious state

Generally under the term "coma" there are three stages of the disorder of consciousness (DoC). DoCs include coma, vegetative state (VS), and minimal consciousness state (MCS). The Glasgow Coma Scale is commonly used immediately after a coma is diagnosed, in the emergency department, and during admission [117], [118]. This scale helps track improvements and deterioration in brain recovery and predict recovery. A fully conscious person has a Glasgow coma score of 15, and a deep coma patient has a

Divided mind and the separated hemispheres

Since its inception more than fifty years ago, split-brain research has continually found itself on the edge of the needle in the development of psychology and neuroscience. The term “split brain” was coined in the mid-20th century to describe the result of a callosotomy, a surgical incision of the corpus callosum to relieve severe epilepsy in patients. During this procedure, the corpus callosum - the bundle of nerve fibers that connects the left and right hemispheres of the brain - is cut to

Technical aspects of assessing the level of consciousness

The neurobiological aspects of the problem of consciousness have direct application in clinical practice. The advances in resuscitation medicine make the request for a certain technical "consciousness meter" - a fairly reliable technique based on tracking physiological correlates of mental activity [12]. Such a technique still does not exist, and until recently, the patient's level of consciousness was assessed on the basis of his ability to interact with society.

Indeed, observing purposeful

Conclusion

One of the main questions of neurobiology is the question of localization within the neural circuits of the brain, which is the carrier of consciousness. In other words, neuroscientists have long attempted to figure out where the individual's ability to experience internal and external sensations is localized in the brain. Probably, certain brain structures play a key role in these processes. The question of the structures of the brain that are critical for the functioning of consciousness is a

References (173)

  • P. Redgrave et al.

    Anticonvulsant role of nigrotectal projection in the maximal electroshock model of epilepsy-II. Pathways from substantia nigra pars lateralis and adjacent peripeduncular area to the dorsal midbrain

    Neuroscience

    (1992)
  • S. Bodovitz

    The neural correlate of consciousness

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2008)
  • J.B. Smith et al.

    The claustrum

    Curr. Biol.

    (2020)
  • Y. Goll et al.

    Attention: the claustrum

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2015)
  • G. Tononi et al.

    Schizophrenia and the mechanisms of conscious integration

    Brain Res. Rev.

    (2000)
  • M.Z. Koubeissi et al.

    Electrical stimulation of a small brain area reversibly disrupts consciousness

    Epilepsy Behav.

    (2014)
  • M. Tallent et al.

    The cloned kappa opioid receptor couples to an N-type calcium current in undifferentiated PC-12 cells

    Neuroscience

    (1994)
  • D. Peckys et al.

    Expression of MU, KAPPA, and delta opioid receptor messenger RNA in the human CNS: a 33P in situ hybridization study

    Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • M.J. Baggott et al.

    Use patterns and self-reported effects of Salvia divinorum: an internet-based survey

    Drug Alcohol Depend.

    (2010)
  • A.E. Cavanna et al.

    Epileptic seizures and spirit possession in Haitian culture: report of four cases and review of the literature

    Epilepsy Behav.

    (2010)
  • D.J. Englot et al.

    Consciousness and epilepsy: why are complex-partial seizures complex?

    Prog. Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • H. Blumenfeld

    Impaired consciousness in epilepsy

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2012)
  • H. Blumenfeld

    Epilepsy and the consciousness system: transient vegetative state?

    Neurol. Clin.

    (2011)
  • N. Palomero-Gallagher et al.

    Functional organization of human subgenual cortical areas: Relationship between architectonical segregation and connectional heterogeneity

    Neuroimage

    (2015)
  • E. Mayr

    Cause and effect in biology

    Science

    (1961)
  • K.N. Laland et al.

    Cause and effect in biology revisited: is Mayr’s proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful?

    Science

    (2011)
  • B. Zivkovic

    The NEw Meanings of How and Why in Biology?

    Sci. Am.

    (2011)
  • D.J. Chalmers

    How can we construct a science of consciousness?

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2013)
  • S. Paulson et al.

    The thinking ape: the enigma of human consciousness

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2013)
  • A. Scheinin, O. Kantonen, M. Alkire, J. Långsjö, R.E. Kallionpää, K. Kaike, L. Radek, J. Johansson, N. Sandman, M....
  • G.M. Edelman

    The Remembered Present: A Biological Theory of Consciousness

    (1994)
  • G. Tononi et al.

    Consciousness and complexity

    Science

    (1998)
  • L.W. Swanson et al.

    Foundational model of structural connectivity in the nervous system with a schema for wiring diagrams, connectome, and basic plan architecture

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (2010)
  • M. Maldonato

    The ascending reticular activating system: the common root of consciousness and attention

  • C. Koch

    What is consciousness?

    Nature

    (2018)
  • S.H. Jang et al.

    Ascending reticular activating system recovery in a patient with brain injury

    Neurology

    (2015)
  • T. Zhao et al.

    Consciousness: new concepts and neural networks

    Front. Cell. Neurosci.

    (2019)
  • B.E. Jones

    Arousal systems

    Front. Biosci.

    (2003)
  • D.J. Englot et al.

    Functional connectivity disturbances of the ascending reticular activating system in temporal lobe epilepsy

    J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry

    (2017)
  • O. Gosseries et al.

    Disorders of consciousness: what’s in a name?

    NeuroRehabilitation

    (2011)
  • S.S. Yeo et al.

    The ascending reticular activating system from pontine reticular formation to the thalamus in the human brain

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • P. Fuller et al.

    Reassessment of the structural basis of the ascending arousal system

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (2011)
  • W.W. Bao et al.

    Nucleus accumbens neurons expressing dopamine D1 receptors modulate states of consciousness in sevoflurane anesthesia

    Curr. Biol.

    (2021)
  • K. Sasaki et al.

    Pharmacogenetic modulation of orexin neurons alters sleep/wakefulness states in mice

    PLOS One

    (2011)
  • A. Inutsuka et al.

    The physiological role of orexin/hypocretin neurons in the regulation of sleep/wakefulness and neuroendocrine functions

    Front. Endocrinol.

    (2013)
  • C. Koch et al.

    Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2016)
  • M. Ibbotson et al.

    Origins of functional organization in the visual cortex

    Front. Syst. Neurosci.

    (2020)
  • F. Attneave et al.

    The organization of behavior; a neuropsychological theory

    Am. J. Psychol.

    (1950)
  • S. Löwel et al.

    Selection of intrinsic horizontal connections in the visual cortex by correlated neuronal activity

    Science

    (1992)
  • P.S. Goldman-Rakic

    Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1996)
  • View full text