Substance use to enhance academic performance among Australian university students

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peh.2013.08.017Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Australian students use a range of substances to enhance academic performance.

  • The main reasons for use are to improve focus/concentration, or to stay awake.

  • It is unclear if cognitive enhancing drug use is a performance or health issue.

Abstract

Use of substances to enhance academic performance among university students has prompted calls for evidence to inform education and public health policy. Little is known about this form of drug use by university students outside the US. A convenience sample of n = 1729 Australian university students across four universities responded to an exploratory on-line survey. Students were asked about their lifetime use of modafinil, prescription stimulants (e.g. methylphenidate), supplements (e.g. ginkgo biloba), illicit drugs (e.g. speed), relaxants (e.g. valium) and caffeine in relation to enhancing study performance. The results show that Australian students report using substances for study purposes at a higher lifetime rate than observed among US or German students. The main reasons for use were to improve focus and attention, and to stay awake. Use of substances to enhance study outcomes was correlated with faculty of study, attitude and use of other substances. These results point to the need to develop Australian evidence to guide policy or regulatory responses to student use of substances to enhance academic performance.

Introduction

The evidence base to suggest that students are using various substances to enhance their cognitive performance is growing (Housden, Morein-Zamir, & Sahakian, 2011, chap. 7), prompting calls for regulation by educational (Greely et al., 2008) and public health policy makers (Smith & Farah, 2011). Substances used by students to enhance academic performance include stimulants such as caffeine (CAF), methylphenidate (MPH) and modafinil (MOD). In addition to the ethical harms that may arise from such substance use (e.g. coercion, authenticity and justice; Sandberg & Savulescu, 2011, chap. 6), public health discourses argue significant known health risks exist (e.g. caffeine toxicity; Reissig, Strain, & Griffiths, 2009) alongside unknown health risks (e.g. long term off-label use of MPH or MOD; McCabe and Teter, 2007, Teter et al., 2010). The evidence base for substance use to enhance academic performance is largely constrained to data from the United States (US) examining the non-medical use of prescription stimulants (NMUPS) such as MPH among college students, prompting calls for data to be collected outside the US (Smith and Farah, 2011, Teter et al., 2006) and using different methods (DeSantis et al., 2008, Rabiner et al., 2009). Lucke and Hall (2012) indicate the need for such research in the Australian context is pressing. The current paper reports data from an exploratory study of Australian university student use of substances to enhance academic performance.

Substance use by students to enhance academic performance is commonly thought of as cognitive enhancement, also described as neuroenhancement, cosmetic neurology or brain doping (Partridge, Bell, Lucke, Yeates, & Hall, 2011). Cognitive enhancement is generally thought of as “the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind, using augmentation or improvements of information processing systems” (Sandberg, 2011, chap. 5, p. 72). The cognitive skills enhanced have been defined as perception, attention, memory, comprehension, use of speech and executive function (planning, problem solving and self monitoring) (Housden et al., 2011, chap. 7, p. 113). Cognitive enhancement includes tools (e.g. an abacus), training (e.g. study), brain–computer interfaces (e.g. cochlear implants) and new senses (e.g. magnetic field perception) (Sandberg, 2011, chap. 5). It also includes natural substances demonstrated to influence cognition (e.g. CAF) along with pharmaceutically refined (e.g. Omega 3 (OM3)) or derived substances (e.g. MPH and MOD). Pharmaceutically derived substances are typically developed to treat neurological conditions such as attention deficit disorder or narcolepsy (Lanni et al., 2008) and adapted for cognitive enhancement through diversion to off-label use (Greely et al., 2008).

There is mixed evidence that the substances presumed to influence cognitive function and enhance academic performance do achieve this intention, and no evidence it meets the inflated claims of effect made in some parts of the literature (see Schermer, Bolt, de Jongh, & Olivier, 2009). While CAF has a long history of human physical and mental performance enhancement, there is mixed evidence of the effects of nutritional supplements Ginkgo Biloba (GBI) and Omega-3 (OM3) as cognition enhancers (e.g. Elsabagh et al., 2005, Kalmijn et al., 1997, Kennedy et al., 2009, Snitz et al., 2009). There are equally mixed results reported in contrasting reviews of pharmaceutically derived cognitive enhancers. There is general agreement that such substances rarely improve performance across the spectrum of cognition, and instead have niche effects for learning over time (Smith & Farah, 2011) or overcoming cognitive impairments due to sleep deprivation (Repantis, Schlattman, Laisney, & Heuser, 2010). One suggestion for such variability has been the lack of controlled randomised studies to assess the effects accounting for dose response, genetics, personality, prevailing ability and task characteristics (Smith & Farah, 2011). In the absence of reliable evidence that the substances do enhance cognition, the issue may be less about actual cognitive enhancement, and more on the belief of enhancement (Repantis et al., 2010). Irrespective of actual or perceived effect, an observable trend towards increasing use of these substances for performance enhancing purposes has emerged.

The majority of data on the use of cognitive enhancing substances is from public health research exploring the NMUPS among US college students. A systematic review of US data by Wilens et al. (2007) put past year non-medical use of prescription stimulants (NMUPS) by college students at between 5% and 35%. For example, where McCabe, Knight, Teter, and Wechsler's (2005) survey of n = 10,904 US college students showed lifetime and past year rates for NMUPS at 6.9%, the multimethod study reported by DeSantis et al. (2008) yielded a 34% lifetime rate (n = 175). By comparison, a study of n = 1547 German high school and university students found the lifetime rate of NMUPS for cognitive enhancement was 1.3% (Franke et al., 2011). A small study of n = 77 Italian university students reported a lifetime use of 16% (Castaldi et al., 2012). By comparison, a survey of prescription drug use to enhance normal level of concentration or alertness in the Australian general population found a lifetime use rate of 2.4% (Partridge, Lucke, & Hall, 2012). The variation in NMUPS lifetime use rates suggests results may be sensitive to context. The evidence base for policy discussion in the Australian context therefore needs to be expanded to account for local conditions (Lucke and Hall, 2012, Partridge et al., 2013).

The current study started as a pilot for a larger scale investigation of substance use to enhance academic performance by Australian university students. The unexpectedly enthusiastic response from n = 1729 Australian university students saw the study change in scope from a pilot to exploratory. The sample size enables the study to give a general indication of prevalence and correlates for substance use to enhance academic performance to inform future research.

Section snippets

Sampling frame

Following approval from two university human research ethics committees (HREC), students at four large South Eastern Australian universities were invited to participate in the online “Study Drugs Study”. The universities represented a mix of research and teaching intensive, and urban and rural campuses. Use of an online convenience sample represents a cost-effective way to collect data about stigmatised behaviour such as drug use appropriate to the original pilot study design. While the sample

Characteristics of the samples

A convenience sample of n = 1729 students responded. Demographic characteristics of all university students in Australia in 2011 are drawn from data collected by the Australian Government (2013); no age data was available. The sample had slightly more females (53.2%, n = 15 missing, AUS 55.7%), was 23.9 years old (SD = 7.2, range 18–70, n = 35 missing) and majority undergraduate (80.0%, AUS 70.5%); 16.9% were post-graduate coursework and 3.1% research students (n = 101 missing) (AUS 24.7% and 4.8%

Discussion

Enough Australian university students use substances to enhance academic performance to warrant further investigation. However, the results are caveat to the exploratory nature of the study. The relative lifetime use rates of stimulants are higher than those reported from the US and Germany, suggesting higher rates of use than Italian university students and the Australian general population. Australian students appear to use substances for an immediate effect (e.g. improve concentration)

Conclusions

In response to Lucke and Hall (2012), this exploratory study demonstrates the need to gather more evidence of Australian university student use of substances to enhance academic performance. This evidence is vital towards establishing education policy or possibly regulation of this class of substance use. The first question to be resolved is whether this issue is dealt with as one of health or one of performance enhancement (cf Forlini & Racine, 2009). The former can be argued in terms of

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating Universities for giving access and the students who so generously gave their time.

References (52)

  • C.M. Barnes et al.

    Borrowing from sleep to pay work and family: Expanding time-based conflict to the broader nonwork domain

    Personnel Psychology

    (2012)
  • V. Cakic

    Smart drugs for cognitive enhancement: Ethical and pragmatic considerations in the era of cosmetic neurology

    Journal of Medical Ethics

    (2009)
  • S. Castaldi et al.

    Use of cognitive enhancement medication among Northern Italian university students

    Journal of Addiction Medicine

    (2012)
  • P. Cross et al.

    Innovative techniques for estimating illegal activities in a human–wildlife-management conflict

    PLoS ONE

    (2013)
  • A.D. DeSantis et al.

    Illicit use of prescription ADHD medications on a college campus: A multimethodological approach

    Journal of American College Health

    (2008)
  • P. Dietz et al.

    Randomized response estimates for the 12-month prevalence of cognitive-enhancing drug use in university students

    Pharmacotherapy

    (2013)
  • J.L. Ebejer et al.

    Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Australian adults: Prevalence, persistence, conduct problems and disadvantage

    PLoS ONE

    (2012)
  • S. Elsabagh et al.

    Differential cognitive effects of Ginkgo biloba after acute and chronic treatment in healthy young volunteers

    Psychopharmacology

    (2005)
  • C. Forlini et al.

    Disagreements with implications: Diverging discourses on the ethics of non-medical use of methylphenidate for performance enhancement

    BMC Medical Ethics

    (2009)
  • A.G. Franke et al.

    Non-medical use of prescription stimulants and illicit use of stimulants for cognitive enhancement in pupils and students in Germany

    Pharmacopsychiatry

    (2011)
  • M. Galesic et al.

    Effects of questionnaire length on participation and indicator of response quality in a web survey

    Public Opinion Quarterly

    (2009)
  • J. Graham et al.

    Mapping the moral domain

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • H. Greely et al.

    Towards responsible use of cognition enhancing drugs by the healthy

    Nature

    (2008)
  • K.M. Hall et al.

    Illicit use of prescribed stimulant medication among college students

    Journal of American College Health

    (2005)
  • C.R. Housden et al.

    Cognitive enhancing drugs: Neuroscience and society

  • R. Judson et al.

    Illicit use of prescription stimulants among college students: Prescription status, theory of planned behaviour, knowledge and self-diagnostic tendencies

    Psychology, Health & Medicine

    (2009)
  • Cited by (42)

    • “A reward for surviving the day”: Women professionals’ substance use to enhance performance

      2022, Performance Enhancement and Health
      Citation Excerpt :

      Here we explore the ways sociocultural performance expectations attached to gender affect the substance use experiences of women professionals. If substances are indeed used to enhance performance or to mitigate performance decrements (Mazanov et al., 2013)(Mazanov, Dunn, Connor, & Fielding, 2013), as Maslen (2013) so cogently asks in an entirely different context, “enhancement to what end?” ( p. 3) What is the performance at stake when professional women, and mothers, use substances “for surviving the day” or so they “can actually function, parent, get to work, and do what [they] need there”?

    • Rethinking enhancement substance use: A critical discourse studies approach

      2021, International Journal of Drug Policy
      Citation Excerpt :

      The consumption of these substances is understood to be motivated to improve physical performance and appearance, the desire for a muscular physique, and improved recovery from training and injuries (Hanley Santos and Coomber, 2017; Murray et al, 2016; Salinas et al, 2019; Van Hout & Kean, 2015); and 2) cognitive enhancers, typically prescription stimulants like Adderall and modafinil – being used without prescription - among student populations (Cakic, 2009; Mazanov et al. 2013; Vargo and Petróczi, 2016). The motivations for consuming cognitive enhancers include to improve focus, attention and productivity, to stay awake, to catch up with work and ultimately to improve performance in university assessments (Cakic, 2009; Mazanov et al. 2013; Vargo and Petróczi, 2016) or the workplace (Keane, 2011). These two substance use consumption practices typically result in and are motivated by a desire for self-improvement.

    • Religious faith, academic stress, and instrumental drug use in a sample of Western-African University students

      2018, Performance Enhancement and Health
      Citation Excerpt :

      While NE by means of freely available lifestyle drugs appears to be quite common among university students (up to 89%; Mache, Eickenhorst, Vitzthum, Klapp, & Groneberg, 2012), the reported rates for NE by means of prescription drugs (2.2 - 8%; Forlini, Schildmann, Roser, Beranek, & Vollmann, 2015; McCabe, Knight, Teter, & Wechsler, 2005; Sattler & Wiegel, 2013; Teter, McCabe, Cranford, Boyd, & Guthrie, 2005) or illicit drugs are markedly lower (0.3%–2.3%; Franke et al., 2011; Mache et al., 2012). Most studies have targeted student samples in Western countries and even within Western countries prevalence differences have been found (e.g., Kudlow, Naylor, Xie, & McIntyre, 2013; Mazanov, Dunn, Connor, & Fielding, 2013; Schelle et al., 2015). Few studies have investigated NE behavior in non-Western countries.

    • Pharmacological cognitive enhancement among non-ADHD individuals—A cross-sectional study in 15 countries

      2018, International Journal of Drug Policy
      Citation Excerpt :

      Media articles portrayed PCE among healthy students as common and increasing (Partridge, Bell, Lucke, Yeates, & Hall, 2011), while scientific evidence for such an increase remained weak, (Maier & Schaub, 2015). Although the prevalence of PCE has been addressed in several large scale surveys, cross-cultural comparisons have been hampered by the different definitions used in these studies (Maier, Haug, & Schaub, 2016; Maier & Schaub, 2015; Mazanov, Dunn, Connor, & Fielding, 2013). For example, the lifetime prevalence of the non-medical use of prescription stimulants in the United States varied from 5% to 35% (Wilens et al., 2008), without specifying the purpose.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text