Main

Medications for the treatment of substance abuse may be developed for a variety of purposes (e.g., ameliorate toxicity, treat overdoses) but a primary goal is to attenuate the subjective, behavioral, or physiological effects of the drug of abuse. Buprenorphine is a high affinity, mu opioid partial agonist, with kappa antagonist action (Cowan et al. 1977; Heel et al. 1979; Lewis et al. 1983). This compound is being evaluated as an alternative to methadone maintenance and detoxification in the treatment of opioid dependence (Bickel and Amass 1995). Buprenorphine's mu receptor (μOR) agonist actions could reduce opiate craving and prevent withdrawal, thus improving compliance with treatment compared to conventional opiate receptor antagonists (e.g., naltrexone). At the same time, its partial efficacy and high affinity at μOR's provide long lasting functional antagonism, making it an effective means of blocking the effects of abused opiates. This unique combination of pharmacological properties confers potential advantages, including enhanced safety over existing medications for treating opioid abuse (Lange et al. 1990).

Buprenorphine sublingual (SL) liquid has been shown to decrease heroin self-administration in single-dose human laboratory studies (Mello and Mendelson 1980; Mello et al. 1981, 1982); the dose-dependency of its effects on reinforcement has been less well characterized. In a clinical study, Johnson et al. (1995) compared the short-term efficacy of placebo, 2 and 8 mg/day buprenorphine in a parallel group, randomized design during a two-week initial maintenance period. In this study, active doses were more effective than placebo in decreasing heroin use, however, there were no differences in therapeutic efficacy between 2 and 8 mg. Schottenfeld et al. (1993) used a within-subject study design in which heroin-dependent outpatients received ascending/descending maintenance doses of buprenorphine ranging from 2 to 16 mg/day. Sixteen milligrams produced significant decreases in opioid-positive urine samples, relative to lower doses. Thus, available data suggest that, relative to placebo, buprenorphine doses ranging from 2 to 8 mg are equally capable of decreasing opioid use, whereas doses of 16 mg have greater efficacy. This may suggest that the effect of buprenorphine in decreasing opiate self-administration may not be dose-dependent, at least in lower dose ranges. In fact, in a recently completed laboratory study in heroin-dependent volunteers, maintenance doses of 2, 4, and 8 mg buprenorphine showed equal efficacy in decreasing the reinforcing effects of hydromorphone (Greenwald et al. 1999).

On the other hand, laboratory studies have demonstrated that SL buprenorphine blocks many of the subjective effects of hydromorphone, a full mu opioid agonist, in a dose-dependent manner. For instance, Bickel et al. (1988) showed that maintenance on buprenorphine (2, 4, 8, 16 mg) produced dose-related decreases in subjective effects to cumulative hydromorphone (0, 6, 18 mg subcutaneous) challenges. Rosen et al. (1994) also found that maintenance on buprenorphine 12 mg (relative to 2 and 6 mg) significantly blocked the subjective effects produced by hydromorphone relative to maintenance on 2 and 6 mg.

A central principle of pharmacotherapy of opiate dependence is that the clinical efficacy of medications is directly related to their occupancy of brain opioid receptors. Positron emission tomography (PET) and radiotracers that label opioid receptors have become available to examine this hypothesis. A single preliminary report has shown that participants who were stabilized on methadone (30–90 mg/day) and abstinent from heroin had 22–35% lower opioid receptor availability than healthy controls, 22 hours after the last dose. These data suggest that relatively low degrees of occupancy are needed to induce clinically significant effects (Kling et al. 1997). However, this study was conducted utilizing [18F]cyclofoxy, which is a non-selective μOR and kappa receptor marker (Carson et al. 1993), and therefore the receptor availability measure calculated does not refer to one but two receptor sites. Since methadone has much higher affinity for mu than for kappa receptor sites (Kristensen et al. 1995), the use of a non-selective radiotracer would underestimate changes in μOR availability. Also, the study assumed that healthy subjects and methadone-maintained participants had similar regional μOR binding concentrations, which may not be the case. Similar studies have not been conducted with partial opioid agonists, such as buprenorphine, or with receptor-selective radiotracers.

In this study we present preliminary data on the in vivo availability of μOR's after treatment with SL buprenorphine 2 mg, 16 mg, and after detoxification on 0 mg, under double-blind conditions. Mu opioid receptor binding was examined in three otherwise healthy opioid-dependent volunteers and in age-matched controls with PET and the selective μOR radiotracer [11C]carfentanil.

Methods

Participant Recruiting and Selection

The Institutional Review Boards of Wayne State University and the University of Michigan approved all procedures and experimental protocols. Three opioid-dependent volunteers and three age-matched non-dependent healthy volunteers between the ages of 20 and 50 were recruited through newspaper advertisements. All volunteers provided written informed consent and were paid for their participation. Opioiddependent volunteers were not seeking treatment and willing to participate in a short-term study involving buprenorphine maintenance and detoxification.

A structured interview (SCID-IV) (First et al. 1996) was administered by an experienced, masters' level psychologist, to all volunteers (SCID-IV patient version in the case of heroin-dependent volunteers, SCID-IV non-patient version for healthy control subjects) to rule out the presence of psychiatric illnesses and diagnose opioid dependency. Severity of opioid dependence was determined using the Addiction Severity Index (McLellan et al. 1985a,b). Volunteers were included who were otherwise healthy, and had no present or past history of psychiatric illnesses, except for nicotine dependence, acute medical or neurological illnesses. Volunteers who met DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder were also excluded.

Drug Preparation

Opioid-dependent participants were maintained on ascending, then descending sublingual (SL) buprenorphine (BUP) doses over a six-week period. The liquid formulation (1 ml ped-pods; National Institute on Drug Abuse) was used to administer BUP. These contained BUP concentrations of 2 mg/ml, 4 mg/ml, and 8 mg/ml in 30% ethanol vehicle. Liquid from the ped-pods was transferred to 1 ml syringes (without needles). To maintain double-blind conditions, two syringes per day were used to deliver the medication under the tongue. The 1 mg doses (detoxification phase; see below) were prepared by mixing 1 mg of BUP hydrochloride powder (National Institute on Drug Abuse) with vehicle, and placebo doses/syringes were prepared using only vehicle.

Procedures

Drug Administration

Dependent volunteers received BUP doses in the following order: 2 mg on days 1–10 (followed by a PET scan on day 10); 4 mg on days 11 and 12; 8 mg on days 13 and 14; 16 mg on days 15–24 (followed by a PET scan on day 24); 8 mg on days 25 and 26; 4 mg on days 27 and 28; 2 mg on days 29 and 30; 1 mg on days 31 and 32; and 0 mg (placebo) on days 33–41 (followed by a PET scan on day 41). Administration of BUP took place daily, at approximately the same time. Buprenorphine doses were held under the tongue for 5 min in the presence of a research assistant, after which the volunteer was instructed to swallow any remaining liquid. BUP administration took place on an outpatient basis until four days prior to each PET scanning session, when dependent participants were admitted into an inpatient unit.

Clinical Measures

Observed urine samples for toxicology testing were obtained twice a week during the outpatient period, and daily during the residential stay. Semiquantitative analyses of these urine samples were performed using fluorescence polarization immunoassay (FPIA; Abbott ADx instrument and standard reagents). These were analyzed for levels of opiates, methadone, cocaine, benzodiazepines, and barbiturates. Opiate drug levels were below the standard cutoff value (200 ng/ml) and all other drug levels were negative on the day of each PET scan.

Vital signs (respiration rate, oral temperature, oxygen saturation, heart rate, and non-invasive blood pressure) were measured each inpatient day, before and periodically after daily buprenorphine administration. Opiate symptoms and heroin craving were rated for the three days prior to each PET scan in the inpatient research unit. Opiate agonist and withdrawal symptoms were assessed using a 32-item questionnaire (see Schuster et al. 1995). Sixteen items are related to agonist symptoms (nodding, dry mouth, turning of stomach, skin itchy, relaxed, coasting, pleasant sick, drive/energy, nervous, good mood, high, sleepy, drunken, soapbox/talkative, rush, friendly) and 16 items are related to withdrawal symptoms (yawning, restless, watery eyes, runny nose, sick to stomach, irritable, tense/jittery, chills/gooseflesh, sweating, hot or cold feelings, abdominal cramps, backache, skin clammy/damp, muscle cramps, painful joints, heavy/sluggish feeling). Each item was scored on a scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Drug craving was assessed using the 34-item score from the Heroin Craving Questionnaire (HCQ) developed by Tiffany (see Schuster et al. 1995 for a description).

PET Scanning Procedures

Three PET scans were obtained in opioid-dependent participants starting 4 hrs after the last administration of BUP 2 mg, BUP 16 mg, and placebo (BUP 0 mg), under double-blind conditions. A single PET scan was obtained in non-dependent participants. All volunteers refrained from smoking, caffeine and food intake for three hours prior to scanning. PET scans were acquired with a Siemens ECAT EXACT-47 scanner in 3-D mode (intrinsic FWHM resolution ≈6 mm in-plane and 5 mm in the z axis), with septa retracted. Participants were positioned in the PET scanner gantry using the orbito-meatal line as reference line, and one intravenous (antecubital) line was placed. A light forehead restraint was used to eliminate intrascan movement. [11C]carfentanil was synthesized at high specific activity (> 1000 Ci/mmol) by the reaction of 11C-methyliodide and a non-methyl precursor as previously described (Dannals et al. 1985), with minor modifications to improve its synthetic yield.

Approximately 15 mCi (555 MBq) were administered to each subject per PET scan, with a maximum mass injected of 0.05 μg/kg per study. This ensured that the compound was administered in tracer quantities, i.e., subpharmacological doses. Fifty-five percent of the [11C]carfentanil dose was administered as a bolus, and the remainder as a continuous infusion, using a computer-controlled automated pump to achieve steady-state tracer levels. Sixteen sets of scans were acquired over 70 min with an increasing duration (30 sec up to 10 min). Time points were decay-corrected by a calculated method and reconstructed using Hanning 0.5 filtered back-projection, in a 24 × 24 cm field of view and a 128 × 128 pixel matrix, with scatter correction. Attenuation correction was performed through a 10-min transmission scan (68Ge source) obtained immediately prior to the PET study. Dynamic images were then co-registered using automated computer routines (Minoshima et al. 1992).

Image data were transformed, on a pixel-by-pixel basis, into two sets of parametric maps: 1) a tracer transport measure (K1 ratio), which is proportional to cerebral blood flow (Tracer Transport = Blood Flow × Extraction); and 2) a receptor-related measure. To avoid repeated arterial blood sampling, the tracer transport and binding measures were calculated using a modified Logan graphical analysis (Logan et al. 1996), with the occipital cortex (an area devoid of μOR's) (Frost et al. 1989) as input function (distribution volume ratio, DVR). The Logan plot becomes sufficiently linear by 5 min after the start of radiotracer administration with a slope proportional to the (Bmax/Kd) + 1 for this receptor site (Koeppe 1999), where Bmax/Kd is often referred to as the “binding potential” (Mintun et al. 1984).

K1 images were then aligned to the intercommisural line and non-linear warped to stereotactic coordinates, as previously described (Minoshima et al. 1993, 1994). The transformation matrix was then applied to the coregistered DVR images. K1 and DVR images were analyzed using a template of regions-of-interest (ROIs) placed on various brain structures following Talairach and Tournoux (1988) stereotactic coordinates. ROI's were cubic volumes of 6.75 mm each side positioned first on the K1 images and then applied to the DVR binding maps. ROI's were placed bilaterally in all regions, at three separate levels, following Brodmann definitions, and averaged within each region. Since no lateralization of the effects of buprenorphine was expected, right and left-sided ROI's were again averaged to yield a single regional value.

Mu opioid receptor availability data are expressed as the mean ± 1 s.d. for the DVR minus 1 of each region (referred to as binding potential). In some instances, the percent coefficient of variation (%COV) is also utilized to illustrate variability across participants [%COV = (s.d. × 100)/mean]. Dose-dependent changes in heroin-withdrawal symptoms, craving scores and regional μOR availaibility were examined with independent, one-way repeated measures ANOVA for each region. Baseline receptor binding potential at BUP 0 mg was compared to that of control participants using independent F tests for each region. The minimum level of significance was set at p < .05.

Results

Participant Characteristics and Clinical Measures

Demographic information and drug use of the two groups of participants are shown in Table 1. Mean (± s.d.) opioid withdrawal scores in the opioid-dependent participants were lowest when they were maintained on the higher BUP dose, 16 mg (1.7 ± 1.8), intermediate at the 2 mg dose (3.2 ± 4.0), and highest under placebo conditions (BUP 0 mg, 17.3 ± 8.3) (F(2,4) = 7.08, p < .05). Similar effects were noted for heroin-craving scores (BUP 16 mg, 97 ± 27; BUP 2 mg, 107 ± 21; and BUP 0 mg, 143 ± 14) (F(2,4) = 11.30, p < .03). Opiate agonist effects were not significantly different across BUP doses (p > .05). There were no clinically significant changes in vital signs related to BUP dose, although oxygen saturation showed a dose-related trend (BUP 16 mg, 94.9 ± 1.3%; BUP 2 mg, 95.6 ± 1.1%; BUP 0 mg, 97.0 ± 2.0%).

Table 1 Demographic Information of Participants

Mu Opioid Receptor Availability After Buprenorphine Administration

A summary of the data for the μOR binding measure is presented in Table 2. Compared to the placebo (BUP 0) condition, BUP 2 mg reduced regional μOR availability in the range of 36 to 50%. Administration of BUP 16 mg was associated with reductions in μOR availability in the range of 79 to 95% (Figure 1). Significant dose-dependency was demonstrated for all the brain regions examined, with the exception of the inferofrontal cortex (Brodmann area 11) that only reached trend levels (p = .06) (Table 2).

Table 2 Dose-dependent changes in Mu Opioid Receptor Availability by Sublingual Buprenorphine in Three Heroin-Dependent Volunteers
Figure 1
figure 1

Parametric images of binding potential (Bmax/Kd) (extracted from Logan plot slopes with occipital cortex as input function) in two representative subjects: a non-dependent control (Control), and a matched heroin-dependent volunteer. The latter is shown under placebo conditions (BUP 0), during treatment with 2 mg (BUP 2) and 16 mg (BUP 16) sublingual liquid. Images are scaled so the occipital cortex binding potential, an area devoid of mu opioid receptors, is equal to 1. Five anatomical levels are shown, from superior (left) to inferior (right). The pseudocolor scale utilized is presented on the right side, using a color range depicting binding potential values from 1 to 4

Individual subject variability in regional μOR availability at the two BUP doses studied depended both of the regions examined (higher variability was observed, as expected, in areas with low receptor concentration, i.e., the cerebellum, but also in some other regions with relatively high μOR binding) and the BUP dose. For example, and at the BUP 2 mg dose, %COVs of the proportion of change were as low as 12 and 15% in the anterior cingulate Brodmann areas 32 and 25, respectively, to as high as 35 and 41% in the inferofrontal cortex (prefrontal cortex BA 11) and amygdala, respectively. Less inter-individual variability was noted for the BUP 16 mg condition, with %COVs at or below 10% for regions with higher μOR concentrations, and up to 15% for low μOR binding regions, such as the cerebellum.

Differences in Mu Opioid Receptor Availability Between Controls and Heroin-Dependent Volunteers During the Placebo Condition

The in vivo availability of μOR's was also compared between non-dependent volunteers and heroin-dependent volunteers after withdrawal of BUP (placebo condition) (Figure 1). Higher regional μOR binding potential was generally observed in the heroin-dependent volunteers, which reached statistical significance in the inferofrontal cortex (BA 11) and anterior cingulate regions (BA 25 and 32), even with the small sample studied. Trend differences were obtained in the prefrontal cortex (BA 10) and ventral caudate (including the nucleus accumbens) regions (Table 3).

Table 3 Summary of Binding Data in Three Healthy Non-Dependent Volunteers and Regional Differences with Three Heroin-Dependent Volunteers Studied During Placebo Administration

When the differences in μOR binding were calculated as the percentage difference between the mean of the non-dependent volunteers and the individual values obtained in the heroin-dependent men, a high degree of regional and inter-individual variability was observed. This is reflected by the high standard deviation obtained for these values (Table 3). In the case of the brain regions in which significant group differences were noted, individual differences in binding ranged from −20% to +63% in the pregenual anterior cingulate (BA 32), −7% to +164% in the subgenual anterior cingulate (BA 25), to as high as −13% to +200% in the inferofrontal cortex (BA 11).

Discussion

This study describes regional in vivo μOR binding data obtained in a small number of heroin-dependent volunteers during maintenance on buprenorphine, a μOR partial agonist, and placebo, compared with an equal number of age-matched non-dependent drug-free volunteers. Two main findings are reported: First, buprenorphine maintenance reduced μOR availability, as measured with [11C]carfentanil and PET within subjects, in a dose-related manner (in the range of 36 to 50% for the 2 mg sublingual dose, and in the range of 79 to 95% for the 16 mg dose, depending on the brain regions). Second, μOR binding potential (Bmax/Kd) in heroin-dependent volunteers studied after buprenorphine treatment and two weeks of placebo administration was highly variable between subjects, and significantly increased in paralimbic brain regions, compared to control values in non-dependent volunteers.

The reductions in μOR availability achieved with buprenorphine, as compared to the placebo condition, were higher that those reported using a full agonist (methadone), which ranged between 22 and 35% for fully therapeutic doses (Kling et al. 1997). The differences in the proportion of receptors occupied by therapeutic agents for heroin dependence between the current data and the report by Kling et al. (1997) may be due to various factors. First, it is possible that a larger proportion of receptor occupancy may be required for a partial agonist to induce clinically significant effects. Buprenorphine has been shown to be clinically effective at or above 8 mg (Bickel and Amass 1995; Schottenfeld et al. 1993). However, technical factors may also play a role. The data from Kling et al. (1997) were obtained with a non-selective radiotracer, [11C]cyclofoxy, which binds with high affinity to both μOR and kappa receptor subtypes. Differences in receptor availability during methadone maintenance would be underestimated because methadone has much higher affinity for the μOR than for kappa sites (Kristensen et al. 1995), therefore occupying only one of two receptor sites labeled. Also, in the present report we examined μOR availability within participants at different buprenorphine doses, whereas in that of Kling et al. (1997), participants maintained on widely varying doses were examined once and compared to an untreated control group. Because we observed that heroin-dependent volunteers had generally higher levels of μOR availability during the placebo condition than non-dependent participants, the calculation of receptor binding differences between detoxified volunteers and a control group would underestimate the receptor occupancy by methadone.

Another aspect of these dose-dependent changes in in vivo μOR binding after buprenorphine administration is the relatively high variability in the reductions observed at the lower dose studied (2 mg). While the intrasubject variation in μOR binding at the 16 mg dose was small, and typically within 10% of the mean values (%COV), a higher regional variability was noted at the 2 mg dose. Specifically, the inferofrontal cortex (Brodmann area 11) and the amygdala registered the highest interindividual variability at this dose, with the individual binding values ranging from 31 to 61% and from 20 to 52% below the placebo condition for those two regions, respectively. These regional differences may be due to technical factors (i.e., partial volume averaging in small brain areas), local differences in the proportion of endogenous ligand or regional differences in receptor number and/or affinity. However, the examination of possible regional differences in μOR occupancy by low buprenorphine doses will require both a larger population of volunteers and additional in vitro studies to fully determine its presence and implications.

Even with the very small sample studied in this preliminary report, significant differences in μOR availability in vivo were detected between non-dependent controls and heroin-dependent volunteers during the placebo condition. While generally increased in most brain regions of heroin-dependent volunteers, μOR binding potential was statistically different between groups in the inferior prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 11), anterior cingulate (Brodmann areas 32 and 25), and reached trend significance levels in the prefrontal cortex area 10 and ventral caudate/nucleus accumbens region. These were unexpected results, since experimental animal data have shown that the chronic administration of buprenorphine is associated with a down-regulation of rat forebrain μOR (and concomitant up-regulation of kappa receptors) without changes in affinity (Belcheva et al. 1993). This may also be the case with morphine (Ulibarri et al. 1987; Tempel et al. 1988), albeit upregulatory changes in μOR binding have been described after chronic treatment with heroin (Bolger et al. 1988). However, it should be noted that our measures were obtained after treatment with ascending, then descending doses of buprenorphine, and two weeks of placebo administration. Therefore, these increases may represent a neurobiological correlate of opioid (or less likely, buprenorphine) withdrawal and upregulatory changes in μOR's or its coupling mechanisms. Confirmation of the differences in regional binding between groups and the variability of the data between heroin-dependent volunteers exceeding that of control subjects will require a larger sample size.

If these findings are confirmed, they are likely to be of importance for the study of μOR involvement in heroin-withdrawal. Increases in μOR binding have been associated with craving scores in cocaine-dependent volunteers undergoing cocaine withdrawal (Zubieta et al. 1996). In a larger sample, it would be of interest to evaluate the relationship between μOR binding potential in these regions and measures of withdrawal or craving for opiates. In addition, the high level of interindividual variability in the degree of receptor upregulation observed in heroin-dependent volunteers may reflect prior patterns of heroin or other substance use, pre-existing individual differences (Zubieta et al. 1999), or clinical profiles (i.e., severity of craving). This is an important issue which would need to be explored in larger populations, preferably in a longitudinal, prospective design, specially since it could possibly be associated with the predisposition of individual subjects to use or relapse in the use of heroin or other drugs of abuse. The contribution of other substances of abuse (i.e., nicotine, alcohol, marijuana) to changes in μOR binding will also require additional control groups.

In summary, these preliminary data suggest that buprenorphine induces a dose-related reduction in μOR availability, reaching a high level (≈90%) of receptor occupancy at a dose (16 mg) that has been shown to be clinically effective in treatment studies. Heroin-dependent volunteers recently detoxified also show increases in μOR binding potential relative to controls that are both regionally and individually variable. Future studies will examine the clinical and symptomatic implications of this variability.