Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 98, November 2019, 106023
Addictive Behaviors

Adverse effect of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met met/met genotype in methamphetamine-related executive dysfunction

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.06.012Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Executive dysfunction in methamphetamine users varies by COMT Val158Met genotype.

  • Methamphetamine effects on executive function are seen only in Met/Met carriers.

  • Val carriers have similar executive function irrespective of methamphetamine use.

  • Slower dopamine clearance conferred by Met is a liability in methamphetamine use.

  • COMT-controlled prefrontal dopamine bioavailability impacts methamphetamine injury.

Abstract

Introduction

The Val allele of the Val158Met single-nucleotide polymorphism of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene (COMT) confers greater catabolism of dopamine (DA) in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) than the Met allele. Met/Met homozygotes typically outperform Val-carriers on tests of executive function (EF), perhaps resulting from increased DA bioavailability. Methamphetamine (METH) causes large releases of DA, which is associated with neurotoxicity and executive dysfunction in chronic METH users. We hypothesized that, contrary to its effect in non-METH-using populations, slower DA clearance conferred by Met/Met will relate to worse EF in METH users.

Methods

149 non-Hispanic White men, stratified by METH dependence (METH+/−) and COMT (Val/Val, Val/Met, Met/Met), completed three tests of EF: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop), and Trail Making Test Part B (Trails B). Demographically-adjusted test scores were averaged to create an EF composite T-score. We examined the interaction of METH and COMT on the EF composite and individual test T-scores, controlling for premorbid functioning and alcohol use.

Results

METH group differences in EF were evident only among Met/Met carriers (beta = −9.36, p < .001) but not among Val carriers: Val/Met (beta = −1.38, p = .44) and Val/Val (beta = −4.34, p = .10). These effects were most salient on the WCST.

Conclusions

In the pre-frontal hyperdopaminergic state triggered by methamphetamine, greater DA inactivation conferred by the Val allele may protect against METH-related executive dysfunction, suggesting genetically-driven differences in vulnerability to METH.

Introduction

Heavy, chronic methamphetamine (METH) exposure is associated with central nervous system (CNS) injury (Davidson, Gow, Lee, & Ellinwood, 2001) and neurocognitive deficits (Scott et al., 2007). Human studies in abstinent users have described deficits in executive function, attention, learning and memory, information processing speed, and motor skills (Dean, Groman, Morales, & London, 2013; Scott et al., 2007). Functions localized to the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and frontostriatal connections may be especially vulnerable to METH effects (Chang, Alicata, Ernst, & Volkow, 2007; Bernacer et al., 2013). The PFC plays a critical role in decision-making and inhibitory control (Sakagami, Pan, & Uttl, 2006), with DA as the major neurotransmitter implicated in the evaluation of rewards, maintenance of addictive behaviors, and differences cognitive function (Starr, Fox, Harris, Deary, & Whalley, 2007; Volkow, Fowler, Wang, & Goldstein, 2002).

Although METH-associated CNS injury is evident, METH exposure parameters (e.g., age at first use, total years of use, lifetime amount consumed, route of consumption, and post-acute length of abstinence) often do not inform the degree of impairment seen among people with a history of METH dependence (Cherner, Bousman et al., 2010; Cherner, Suarez et al., 2010; McCann et al., 2008). This suggests individual differences in vulnerability to the effects of METH, which may result from a combination of environmental and genetic factors. Examining genetic variability may offer insight as to how individual differences contribute to risk for cognitive dysfunction in chronic METH use.

Mechanisms of METH-related injury include alterations in dopamine (DA), serotonin, GABA and glutamate systems (Halpin, Collins, & Yamamoto, 2014; McCann et al., 2008). METH principally modulates DA neurotransmission and increases extracellular DA concentrations by a number of means, which include stimulating DA release and inhibiting reuptake via the DA transporter (Lin, Sambo, & Khoshbouei, 2016). In addition to dopaminergic activity in the synapse, an important mechanism of DA-related METH neurotoxicity may occur at the receptor level; for example, a recent study shows that phasic METH-induced DA release impacts D1 DA receptor availability which is negatively associated with cortical thickness (Okita et al., 2017). While DA is critical for cognitive function, overexposure to DA in the synapse caused by stimulant exposure likely plays a role in neural compromise, including damage to DA terminals, microvascular injury, and structural and functional abnormalities on neuroimaging (Dean, Morales, Helleman, & London, 2018; Nordahl, Salo, & Leamon, 2003; Schmidt, Ritter, Sonsalla, Hanson, & Gibb, 1985). Thus, regulatory mechanisms that assist in removing DA from the synapse, play an important role in DA homeostasis in the brain (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006). Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), COMT accounts for >60% of the metabolic degradation of released DA in the PFC (Carboni & Silvagni, 2004; Li et al., 2004; Westerink & Spaan, 1982).

A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) of COMT involves a Val to Met amino acid substitution at codon 158 in the membrane-bound COMT (COMT Val158Met). Due to 40% higher enzymatic activity of the Val compared to Met allele (Chen et al., 2004), homozygote carriers of the Val allele (Val/Val genotype) metabolize PFC DA at a more efficient rate, resulting in lower levels of DA in the synapse, whereas those with Met/Met genotype have the lowest rate of DA clearance, resulting in higher level of DA at the synapse. As METH substantially augments the concentration of extracellular DA, we hypothesized that COMT genotype would be a relevant predictor of brain consequences of METH exposure.

COMT Val158Met has been examined in many contexts relevant to catecholamine function. With regard to cognition, it has been linked most consistently to differences in executive function (Bruder et al., 2005; Wishart et al., 2011), although some controversy remains about the replicability of findings (Barnett, Scoriels, & Munafo, 2008; Goldman, Weinberger, Malhotra, & Goldberg, 2009). In healthy adults, the Val allele has been linked to executive dysfunction (Barnett, Jones, Robbins, & Muller, 2007), whereas the Met allele is associated with enhanced executive function (Barnett et al., 2007; Egan et al., 2001). Some evidence suggests this effect may be specific to men (Egan et al., 2001; Solis-Ortiz, Perez-Luque, Morado-Crespo, & Gutierrez-Munoz, 2010). The Met-associated cognitive advantage is likely due to higher DA bioavailability in the PFC resulting from slower clearance coded by Met. Other findings point to an inverted U-shape relationship between DA activity in the PFC and cognitive performance (Mattay et al., 2003; Tunbridge, Harrison, & Weinberger, 2006) such that the relationship between COMT and PFC function is likely to be context dependent and more complex than a simple dichotomy in which a Val allele is harmful and a Met allele is protective. For example, under conditions of DA excess, such as after METH administration, the greater metabolic activity conferred by Val alleles may be more advantageous in restoring the brain to homeostasis. In an earlier study of COMT Val158Met and executive dysfunction in the context of HIV disease and METH dependence, we found that, regardless of HIV status, individuals with Met/Met genotype had better executive function compared (Wallace, Gudelsky, & Vorhees, 1999) to Val carriers, except if they were METH users, and this effect did not generalize to other cognitive domains (Bousman et al., 2010). Although increased bioavailability of cortical DA associated with the Met/Met genotype is thought to enhance executive function under physiologically normal conditions, in the hyperdopaminergic state induced by METH, slow DA clearance could result in neurotoxicity, possibly via DA auto-oxidation (Moszczynska & Callan, 2017; Riddle, Fleckenstein, & Hanson, 2006; Wallace et al., 1999), thus attenuating any advantage, or posing a liability for executive function in METH-using Met/Met individuals.

Here, we aim to examine whether variability in COMT Val158Met contributes to individual differences in executive deficits reported after heavy chronic METH exposure, with the goal to potentially identify genotype groups that are at higher risk of METH-associated executive dysfunction. In this investigation, we are focusing on a more homogenous sample than in our prior work, reducing variability associated with sex and racial background, as well as HIV status, since HIV can also affect dopaminergic circuitry. Our analyses will examine the main and interactive effects of COMT genotype and METH dependence on a three-test composite of executive function (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop Color-Word Test, and Trail Making Test Part B). Follow-up analyses will examine the effects of COMT genotype and METH dependence on each test of executive function. We hypothesize that, contrary to its effect in the general population, among individuals with METH dependence, slower DA clearance in the PFC conferred by the Met/Met genotype, in conjunction with METH-induced dopaminergic excess, will be associated with worse executive function, while Val carriers will show comparatively better executive function.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 85 METH dependent and 64 non-drug dependent comparison research volunteers evaluated at the University of California, San Diego All were HIV- non-Hispanic White men. We limited our sample to a demographically narrow group for the purpose of genetic analyses, as some sex and race differences in COMT effects and allele frequencies have been reported (e.g., Barnett et al., 2007; González-Castro et al., 2013), and we did not have sufficient numbers of women or non-White

Participant characteristics

Participants were all non-Hispanic White men, ranged in age from 18 to 66 years old (M = 38.7, SD = 10.9), and had an average of 12.6 years of formal education (SD = 2.3). Table 1 provides sample demographic and lifetime substance use characteristics by METH status and COMT genotype (Val/Val, Val/Met, Met/Met) group. Across the six groups, METH+ participants had significantly fewer years of formal education, lower WRAT reading scores, more days since last alcohol use and higher average lifetime

Discussion

Our results suggest genetically influenced differences in vulnerability to METH effects on executive dysfunction. Consistent with literature, healthy participants with Met/Met genotype had better executive function performance than Val carriers, while among METH dependent individuals, the reverse was true. Moreover, the performance of METH+ Val carriers was generally indistinguishable from that of METH- Val carriers. That is, the negative effect of methamphetamine on executive dysfunction was

Disclosures

No conflicts of interest were declared.

Sources of funding

This research was supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse R01 DA26334: COMT Genotype and Risky Decision Making in HIV and Methamphetamine Dependence (M. Cherner) & P50DA26306: Translational Methamphetamine AIDS Research Center (I. Grant).

Acknowledgments

The Translational Methamphetamine AIDS Research Center (TMARC) is supported by Center award P50DA026306 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) and is affiliated with the University of California, San Diego (UCSD), the Sanford-Burnham Medical Discovery Institute (SBMDI), and the University of California, Irvine (UCI). The TMARC comprises: Administrative Coordinating Core (ACC) – Executive Unit: Director – Igor Grant, M.D.; Co-Directors – Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D., Scott L. Letendre,

Conflict of interests

All authors declare they have no conflicts of interest.

References (55)

  • L.E. Halpin et al.

    Neurotoxicity of methamphetamine and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine

    Life Sciences

    (2014)
  • E.A. Kiyatkin et al.

    Acute methamphetamine intoxication: Brain hyperthermia, blood-brain barrier, brain edema, and morphological cell abnormalities

    International Review of Neurobiology

    (2009)
  • J.A. Lee et al.

    Alcohol and the auditory brain-stem response, brain temperature, and blood alcohol curves: Explanation of a paradox

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1990)
  • M. Sakagami et al.

    Behavioral inhibition and prefrontal cortex in decision-making

    Neural Networks

    (2006)
  • J.M. Starr et al.

    COMT genotype and cognitive ability: A longitudinal aging study

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2007)
  • E.M. Tunbridge et al.

    Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • N.D. Volkow et al.

    Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: Insight from imaging studies

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2002)
  • B.H. Westerink et al.

    Simultaneous determination of the formation rate of dopamine and its metabolite 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) in various rat brain areas

    Brain Research

    (1982)
  • P. Yuan et al.

    Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: A meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2014)
  • J.H. Barnett et al.

    Effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on executive function: A meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizophrenia and healthy controls

    Molecular Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • J. Bernacer et al.

    Methamphetamine-induced disruption of frontostriatal reward learning signals: relation to psychotic symptoms

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • C.A. Bousman et al.

    COMT Val158Met polymorphism, executive dysfunction, and sexual risk behavior in the context of HIV infection and methamphetamine dependence

    Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Infectious Diseases

    (2010)
  • C.A. Bousman et al.

    Impact of COMT Val158Met on executive functioning in the context of HIV and methamphetamine

    Neurobehavioral HIV Medicine

    (2010)
  • E. Carboni et al.

    Dopamine reuptake by norepinephrine neurons: Exception or rule?

    Critical Reviews in Neurobiology

    (2004)
  • L. Chang et al.

    Structural and metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse

    Addiction

    (2007)
  • M. Cherner et al.

    Cytochrome P450-2D6 extensive metabolizers are more vulnerable to methamphetamine-associated neurocognitive impairment: Preliminary findings

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2010)
  • A. Chung et al.

    Decreased frontal white-matter integrity in abstinent methamphetamine abusers

    The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2007)
  • Cited by (6)

    • Does chronic use of amphetamine-type stimulants impair interference control? – A meta-analysis

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Smaller performance differences/conflict measures are thought to reflect better interference control. On the neuronal level, interference control heavily draws on the dorsal frontal system, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Adelhöfer and Beste, 2020; Chmielewski and Beste, 2019; Hung et al., 2018; Mückschel et al., 2016), the left anterior insula and parietal regions (Bensmann et al., 2019b; Fitzhugh et al., 2019; Hung et al., 2018). When investigating the chronic effects of ATS on the brain and associated cognitive functions, prefrontal areas and the ACC are most relevant because they are densely innervated and modulated by the serotonergic, dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurotransmitter systems (Iversen et al., 2008; Köhler et al., 2016; Robbins and Arnsten, 2009), which have repeatedly been reported to be altered by ATS and associated with ATS-induced cognitive dysfunctions, especially in the executive domain (Jager et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2018; Lederer et al., 2016; McCann et al., 2008b; Nestor et al., 2011; Nordahl et al., 2002; Roberts and Montgomery, 2015; Roberts et al., 2009).

    • COMT val158met genotype alters the effects of methamphetamine dependence on dopamine and dopamine-related executive function: preliminary findings

      2020, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      These demographically-corrected individual test T scores were averaged within each neurocognitive ability domain to generate domain-specific T scores. Given the relevance of COMT to DAergic activity in the PFC (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006) and our prior findings that COMT interacts with METH status to predict executive function, secondary analyses focused on the relationship between CSF DA and executive function T scores (Cherner et al., 2019). The executive function composite was composed of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test 64-item-computerized version, Stroop Color-Word Test, and Trail Making Test Part B.

    View full text