Elsevier

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

Volume 151, February 2014, Pages 36-42
Applied Animal Behaviour Science

Performance of sheep in a spatial maze is impeded by negative stimuli

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.11.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Exposure to negative stimuli or stress can manifest in general changes in cognitive processing. This study aimed to investigate if a spatial maze task could be used to identify stress-induced differences in the cognitive performance of sheep. Two negative stimuli were used to test the hypothesis. For a negative pre-treatment (‘dog’ pre-treatment), sheep were moved individually to a holding yard at the beginning of the maze where they were exposed to a dog for 3 min, for 5 consecutive days. Alternative to the dog pre-treatment, sheep were moved in small groups to the same holding yard, for the same amount of time, where they received a feed reward (‘food’ pre-treatment). For a during-test stimulus, white noise was played as sheep moved through the maze (‘noise’ treatment). Sixty-four male castrated lambs were allocated to one of four groups: dog and noise, food and noise, dog and no noise, or food and no noise. Sheep traversed the maze on 3 consecutive days and the total time to complete the maze, the number and the duration of errors made were used to assess cognitive performance. Maze results were analysed using GLMM, LMM and linear contrasts. The noise increased both total time (140 s vs. 105 s, P = 0.043) and error time (67 s vs. 56 s, P = 0.044) on day 1. The dog pre-treatment increased error time compared to the food pre-treatment (81 s vs. 41 s, P = 0.041) and tended to increase the number of errors made on day 1 (1.5 errors vs. 1.2 errors, P = 0.057). Neither noise nor dog pre-treatment influenced cognitive performance on days 2 or 3. Results suggest that both stimuli affected cognitive performance in the maze by impeding initial problem solving. The maze used demonstrates the ability to identify differences in cognition.

Introduction

Cognition and affective state are closely intertwined. Cognition can influence the formation of affective states (Scherer, 2001, Desiré et al., 2002), and certain affective states can alter the processing of information (Paul et al., 2005). Exposure to negative stimuli or stress can also manifest in general changes in cognitive processing, affecting problem solving, learning and memory (for a detailed review see Mendl, 1999). Research in recent years has focused on identifying these changes in cognition in relation to affective states, and these tests have the ability to assess an animal's welfare.

Much animal welfare research is currently focused on developing tools to measure appraisal (how an animal perceives a certain stimulus; examples in sheep, Desiré et al., 2004, Desiré et al., 2006, Greiveldinger et al., 2007, Deiss et al., 2009, Greiveldinger et al., 2009), and the influence affective states have on the interpretation of information (cognitive bias; examples include rodents, Harding et al., 2004; birds, Bateson and Matheson, 2007; sheep, Doyle et al., 2010; invertebrates, Bateson et al., 2011; dogs, Burman et al., 2011; primates, Bethell et al., 2012). These methods are particularly valuable at detecting more intricate details of an animal's affective state, as they have the potential to measure the valence of an animal's affective state.

Tests for emotional reactivity and cognitive bias can also be complex to interpret (for review see Mendl et al., 2009). In addition, these complex tests take significant time to train and test, and importantly, some current methods to test for cognitive bias cannot be applied to all individuals [e.g. in sheep (Doyle et al., 2011) and dogs (Muller et al., 2012)], leading to the exclusion of animals from tests. This potentially skews results, as animals may fail to complete training tasks because of factors like reduced coping ability or temperament-inhibiting learning (Pascual-Alonso et al., 2013). Based on this, having standardised tests that assess the effects of environmental stimuli on general cognition can be a valuable tool to measure affective state.

General cognitive changes in response to stress can occur for different reasons. For example, in times of stress animals move into a more automatic method of processing information rather than using cognitive control. As a result, their behaviour becomes more rigid and inflexible, preventing them from solving novel tasks effectively (Toates, 2002, Toates, 2006). An animal's attention may also be drawn from a task as the result of cognitive overload from either a large number of stimuli presented at once, or a particularly arousing stimulus. This attentional shifting can result in poorer task performance (Dukas and Kamil, 2000, Dukas and Kamil, 2001, Lavie, 2005, Shettleworth, 2010).

Both of these influential factors can affect problem solving, the process of learning, and memory formation and recall. For example, social isolation and environmental stressors negatively impact on the cognitive performance of rats in different tasks (Sandstrom and Hart, 2005, Harris et al., 2010, Alliger and Moller, 2011). The memory recall of pigs in a spatial cognition task was reduced by stressful factors including social isolation, exposure to an unfamiliar environment, and unpredictable events (Mendl et al., 1997). Laughlin et al. (1999) supported these results in an associated study, with the authors suggesting that this reduction in performance was the result of a deficit in attention rather than inhibition of memory retrieval. The performance of goats in a visual discrimination task was reduced after relocation to a new environment (Langbein et al., 2006). Cattle seemed to be unable to learn a reversal task when it was associated with a restraint stressor, and authors also noted that the calmer animals may have been more able to make accurate choices (Grandin et al., 1994). Calves displaying a greater level of fear following social isolation had poorer cognitive performance (Lensink et al., 2006). Perceived stressors are also enough to alter cognitive performance. Cattle were more distracted from feeding when they perceived a situation to be more threatening (Welp et al., 2004). Similarly, the grazing behaviour of sheep became increasingly interrupted with increased perceived risk of predation (Dumont and Boissy, 2000).

The aim of the current study was to investigate if a spatial task could be used to identify stress-induced differences in the cognitive performance of sheep. A spatial maze task designed by Lee and colleagues (2006) was used in the current study. It has previously been validated to assess spatial learning and memory, requires no prior training and relies on the innate flocking behaviours of sheep. Two stimuli were used to elicit a negative state in the sheep. The ‘dog’ pre-treatment was delivered in the 5 days prior to the maze task with the aim of inducing a negative affective state prior to the commencement of the maze task. The second was a novel auditory stimulus, white noise (‘noise’). The novelty of a stimulus is a key component when forming an emotional response to it, and negative emotions like fear, anxiety and displeasure are associated with unfamiliar stimuli (Scherer, 2001, Desiré et al., 2002). In support of this, stress-related behavioural and physiological responses to novelty have been demonstrated in sheep (Desiré et al., 2006), and more specifically, white noise can generate an increased heart rate in naive sheep (Ames and Arehart, 1972). With this in mind, it was hypothesised that the noise stimulus would impede task performance more than the dog pre-treatment, as the sheep would be exposed to it while performing the task. Some sheep were exposed to both stimuli, and it was hypothesised that these animals would display the poorest cognitive performance.

Section snippets

Methods

The Charles Sturt University Animal Care and Ethics Committee, in accordance with the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes, approved all procedures in this experiment (protocol number 10/096).

Number of completions

There were no significant influences of noise, pre-treatment or day on the probability of sheep completing the maze. Results for successful completions are as follows: day 1 = 77%, day 2 = 81%, day 3 = 91% (F2,187.0 = 2.14, P = 0.12); noise = 84%, no noise = 83% (F1,187.0 = 0.03, P = 0.85); dog = 85%, food = 83% (F1,187.0 = 0.14, P = 0.71). Only one individual failed to complete the maze on all 3 days.

Total time

There was a significant effect of day on total time taken to complete the maze (F2,144.1 = 39.98, P < 0.001), and completion

Discussion

The results from this study demonstrate that the cognitive processing of sheep was impaired by both noise and dog treatments on day 1 of testing, providing evidence that this spatial maze may be a useful method of identifying the effect of stress on cognition. It was not possible to identify which stimulus was more stressful however, and so the hypothesis that the stimulus delivered during the maze (white noise) would be more adverse than the dog pre-treatment cannot be confirmed. There was

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Brian Alston, Brad Kelk, Alyssa Trotter and Grace Hardgraves for their help with the project.

References (49)

  • L. Greiveldinger et al.

    Emotional experience in sheep: predictability of a sudden event lowers subsequent emotional responses

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2007)
  • L. Greiveldinger et al.

    Behavioural and physiological responses of lambs to controllable vs uncontrollable aversive events

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2009)
  • C.E. Hernandez et al.

    Effects of litter size, sex and periconceptional ewe nutrition on side preference and cognitive flexibility in the offspring

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • K. Laughlin et al.

    Disturbance effects of environmental stimuli on pig spatial memory

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (1999)
  • N. Lavie

    Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • C. Lee et al.

    Development of a maze test and its application to assess spatial learning and memory in merino sheep

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (2006)
  • J. Lensink et al.

    Enhancement of performances in a learning task in suckler calves after weaning and relocation: motivational versus cognitive control? A pilot study

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (2006)
  • M. Mendl

    Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (1999)
  • M. Mendl et al.

    Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (2009)
  • M. Mendl et al.

    Pigs in space: spatial memory and its susceptibility to interference

    Anim. Behav.

    (1997)
  • M. Pascual-Alonso et al.

    Identity profiles based on social strategies, morphology, physiology, and cognitive abilities in goats

    J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res.

    (2013)
  • E.S. Paul et al.

    Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2005)
  • N.J. Sandstrom et al.

    Isolation stress during the third postnatal week alters radial arm maze performance and corticosterone levels in adulthood

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • F. Toates

    Application of a multilevel model of behavioural control to understanding emotion

    Behav. Process.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (9)

    • Use of a maze test to assess spatial learning and memory in cattle: Can cattle traverse a complex maze?

      2016, Applied Animal Behaviour Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus the overall completion rate for the 29 cows used in the present study is estimated at 0.21 (=6.21/29). This estimate is much lower than the completion rate of 0.71 for barrows (Sus domesticus) subjected to a maze with five dead ends (Jansen et al., 2009) and 0.77–0.91 for sheep subjected to a maze with two error zones (Doyle et al., 2014). Variation in the success rate in a maze test can be attributed to a number of factors such as animal species, complexity of maze configuration, motivation to traverse the maze and method and frequency of training.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text