Elsevier

Body Image

Volume 27, December 2018, Pages 10-20
Body Image

Psychological predictors of body image attitudes and concerns in young children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.08.005Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Six year old girls and boys were followed-up at 7 years old.

  • Relationships between psychological variables and body image outcomes were examined.

  • Self-esteem and perfectionism were correlated with a number of body image outcomes.

  • Prospectively, internalization and perfectionism predicted higher body dissatisfaction.

  • Psychological attributes should be considered in etiological models of body image.

Abstract

We aimed to examine cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between psychological characteristics, body size, and body image outcomes in 6- to 7-year-old children. Participants were 202 children (91 boys, 111 girls) assessed when 6 and 7 years old. Body image-related variables, body mass index, and psychological characteristics were assessed. Cross-sectionally in the total sample, lower self-esteem was associated with greater body dissatisfaction, internalization of appearance ideals, and rewards of muscularity. Greater perfectionism was associated with greater body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, drive for muscularity, and rewards of thinness and muscularity. Prospectively, internalization and socially prescribed perfectionism at 6 predicted greater body dissatisfaction at 7; higher perfectionism predicted greater rewards of thinness; higher perfectionism and lower self-esteem predicted greater rewards of muscularity; and lower self-esteem and smaller perceived body size predicted higher internalization. Exploratory gender specific analyses suggest that self-esteem may play a greater role in the development of body image attitudes in girls than boys, while perfectionism appears to increase boys’ vulnerability to perceive greater rewards from thinness. Findings support the inclusion of psychological characteristics in etiological models of early body image development, and highlight perfectionism, internalization of appearance ideals, and self-esteem as potential targets for preventative interventions in this age group.

Introduction

Body image attitudes are established during early childhood(Paxton & Damiano, 2017). By age 3, children have frequently internalized body size stereotypes (Harringer, Calogero, Witherington, & Smith, 2010; Spiel, Paxton, & Yager, 2012), and children as young as 5 have expressed body dissatisfaction (Davison, Markey, & Birch, 2000) and engage in dietary restraint (Carper, Fisher, & Birch, 2000; Damiano, Paxton, Wertheim, McLean, & Gregg, 2015). Findings from pre-adolescent and adolescent research indicate that psychological characteristics, including negative affect (Bearman, Presnell, Martinez, & Stice, 2006), self-esteem (Paxton, Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2006), perfectionism (Boone, Soenens, & Braet, 2011), and internalization of appearance ideals (Stice & Whitenton, 2002), as well as larger body size (Paxton et al., 2006), may confer risk for the development of body image concerns. These characteristics might also influence the development of body image during early childhood. The aim of this study, therefore, was to provide new insight into psychological risk factors for the development of body image concerns in young children by examining cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between psychological characteristics, body size, and body image attitudes and concerns in children at age 6 and age 7.

The biopsychosocial model proposes that biological factors (e.g., body size), psychological characteristics (e.g., negative affect, low self-esteem, perfectionism), and sociocultural appearance pressures from family, peers, and the media, combine to increase the tendency to internalize or adopt societal body ideals (i.e., internalization of appearance ideals), and to compare one’s appearance with that of others (i.e., appearance comparison), to thereby confer risk for body dissatisfaction and related eating disturbances (Ricciardelli, McCabe, Holt, & Finemore, 2003; Rodgers, Paxton, & McLean, 2014; Wertheim & Paxton, 2012). Empirical support for the influence of biological factors such as body size (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2005; Paxton et al., 2006; Saling, Ricciardelli, & McCabe, 2005) and sociocultural factors (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2005; Stice & Whitenton, 2002) on body image development has been well established in pre-adolescent and adolescent populations. Evidence for the role of psychological factors in this age group is growing (Boone et al., 2011; Rodgers et al., 2014), and is reviewed below.

Negative affect, defined as a general factor of emotional distress comprising depressive and anxious symptoms (Joiner, Cantazaro, & Laurent, 1996), has been proposed to increase risk of body dissatisfaction by increasing negative attributional bias (Beck, 1976), thereby elevating the likelihood of evaluating one’s own body shape and weight negatively (Bearman et al., 2006; Stice & Whitenton, 2002). Cross-sectional studies have frequently observed positive associations between anxious or depressive symptoms and body dissatisfaction in adolescents (Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; Ohring, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002; Rodgers et al., 2014) and pre-adolescents (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003; Phares, Steinberg, & Thompson, 2004). Prospective studies have consistently found that negative affect predicts increases in body dissatisfaction in adolescent boys (Bearman et al., 2006; Paxton et al., 2006), but not always in girls (Paxton et al., 2006; Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004). No support has been found for either cross-sectional or longitudinal associations between negative affect and dieting in children aged 8–10 years (Saling et al., 2005). However, higher levels of depressive symptoms in 7-year-old girls predict dieting at age 9 (Sinton & Birch, 2005). Whilst methodological differences in measurement, sampling, and time frames examined might partially account for discrepancies between findings, these findings suggest that the role of negative affect as a risk factor for body image concerns might differ according to developmental stage and gender.

Few studies have investigated the relationship between negative affect and body image during early childhood, except one study that found negative affect at ages 3 and 4 predicted drive for thinness in early adolescent girls (but not boys) (Martin et al., 2000). However, anxiety is prevalent in early childhood (Rapee, Schniering, & Hudson, 2009), and anxious and depressive symptoms may arise in children as young as 3 years (Luby, 2013). Negative affect might, therefore, play a role in the development of body image during this time. Specifically, young children high in anxiety might be more self-conscious in social settings, and more sensitive to perceived negative evaluation than others, while those high in depressive symptoms might also develop a more negative view of themselves than others (Huberty, 2012). Heightened sensitivity to perceived criticism and negative self-evaluation may render children more sensitive to appearance-related criticism.

Low self-esteem, a poor global regard for oneself as a person (Harter, 1993), is also theorized to influence the development of body image by increasing vulnerability to internalize appearance ideals and engage in upward appearance comparisons (Durkin, Paxton, & Sorbello, 2007). Conversely, higher self-esteem might mitigate the negative effects of internalization and appearance comparison, thereby reducing risk of body dissatisfaction (Mitchell, Petrie, Greenleaf, & Martin, 2012).

Cross-sectional studies consistently find associations between low self-esteem and body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint in adolescents (Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; Muris, Meesters, van de Blom, & Mayer, 2005) and pre-adolescents (Lawrence & Thelen, 1995; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003; Phares et al., 2004). These findings have generally revealed stronger associations between low self-esteem and body dissatisfaction in females than males (e.g., Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; Muris et al., 2005). However, prospective results have been inconsistent (Paxton et al., 2006; Tiggemann, 2005; Wojtowicz & von Ranson, 2012). Self-esteem has been found to be correlated with body satisfaction in 8- to 10-year-old children, but no evidence has been found for a prospective association between self-esteem and body satisfaction in this age-group (Mendelson, White, & Mendelson, 1996). Saling et al. (2005) also found no prospective relationship between self-esteem and dieting in pre-adolescent children; however, self-esteem predicted muscle preoccupation in pre-adolescent girls but not boys. These findings suggest that self-esteem might influence the development of body image concerns in males and females differently at varying stages of development.

Little is currently known about the impact of self-esteem on body image development during early childhood. However, research suggests that young children (3–7 years old) generally report high self-esteem (Harter, 2006). Maturational changes in cognitive development that occur during middle childhood have been proposed to introduce liabilities that lead to increased negative self-evaluation and decreased self-esteem for some children (Harter, 2006).

Perfectionism has also been both theoretically (Wertheim & Paxton, 2012) and empirically (Bardone-Cone et al., 2007; Boone et al., 2011) linked to body dissatisfaction and eating symptomatology. Growing consensus indicates that perfectionism is a multidimensional construct broadly conceptualized as comprising two main dimensions: one involving self-imposed expectations of unrealistically high standards (described as self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) or personal standards perfectionism), and the other involving the perceived need to attain high standards prescribed by others and excessive concerns about others’ disapproval (described as socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP) or evaluative concerns perfectionism) (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991). Individuals who apply inflexible evaluative criteria to themselves are proposed to also apply such criteria to their bodies, thereby internalizing sociocultural appearance pressures (Boone, Soenens, & Luyten, 2014). Perfectionism might further predispose individuals to pursue and base their self-worth on achieving unrealistic body image ideals, thereby increasing their risk of body dissatisfaction and unhealthy weight control behaviors (Cash, 2002; Grammas & Schwartz, 2009; Wojtowicz & von Ranson, 2012).

In support of this theory, a composite measure of SOP and SPP has been found to be positively correlated with body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls (Teixeira, Pereira, Marques, Saraiva, & de Macedo, 2016), whilst SOP and SPP have been associated with dysfunctional eating in adolescents (Bento et al., 2010; Teixeira et al., 2016). Self-oriented perfectionism has also been associated with disordered eating in 12-year-old girls (McVey, Pepler, Davis, Flett, & Abdolell, 2002). In pre-adolescent children, a composite measure of SOP and SPP has been found to correlate with muscle preoccupation in boys, and dieting and muscle concerns in girls (Saling et al., 2005).

Longitudinally, in adolescents, evaluative concerns perfectionism has predicted increased drive for thinness (Dickie, Wilson, McDowall, & Surgenor, 2012) and increases in bulimic symptoms both directly and indirectly through perceived pressure to be thin, internalization of the thin ideal, and body dissatisfaction (Boone et al., 2011). However, Boone et al. (2014) later found no direct prospective association between perfectionism and body dissatisfaction. In pre-adolescents, Saling et al. (2005) found that a composite measure of SOP and SPP predicted dieting and muscle preoccupation in 8- to 10-year-old boys.

To date, however, the relationship between perfectionism and body image concerns in children younger than 8 years old has not been investigated. Evidence suggests that early childhood may mark a key period in developing perfectionistic traits (Evans et al., 1997; Flett, Hewitt, Oliver, & Macdonald, 2002). Parental influences such as modeling exacting standards and self-evaluative tendencies, as well as child temperament and a sense that self-worth is contingent upon meeting others’ expectations, have been proposed to influence childhood development of SPP (Flett et al., 2002). The development of SOP is further proposed to require children to actively internalize or incorporate socially imposed standards into pressures on the self (Flett et al., 2002). Perfectionism developed during early childhood might, therefore, increase children’s vulnerability to internalize and emulate unrealistic body ideals and negative self-evaluative tendencies modeled by significant others. Greater perfectionism might also render children vulnerable to viewing their self-worth as being contingent upon achieving societal body image standards. Such vulnerabilities may lead to body dissatisfaction in girls and boys.

Psychological characteristics (i.e., negative affect, low self-esteem, perfectionism) have been proposed to increase risk of internalizing appearance ideals, and in turn lead to body dissatisfaction and associated body change behaviors (Wertheim & Paxton, 2012). Internalization has therefore been theorized to mediate the relationship between psychological factors and body image concerns (Wertheim & Paxton), and may thus be considered both a predictor and outcome variable. Support for the relationship between internalization and body dissatisfaction is evident in adolescent cross-sectional (Halliwell & Harvey, 2006; Knauss, Paxton, & Alsaker, 2007) and longitudinal investigations (Stice & Whitenton, 2002). Support for the role of internalization in mediating the relationship between psychological variables including negative affect (Durkin et al., 2007; Rodgers et al., 2014) and perfectionism (Boone et al., 2011) and body image concerns has also been found in adolescents.

Little is currently known about the influence of internalization on body image development during early childhood. However, evidence of thin-ideal internalization has been revealed in 3- to 5-year-old children (Damiano et al., 2015; Harringer et al., 2010; Spiel et al., 2012). Internalization has also been shown to correlate with dietary restraint in 5-year-old girls (Damiano et al., 2015). As the early school years mark a time when children are increasingly exposed to sociocultural influences from outside the family, internalization may play an important influence in developing children’s body image concerns during this period.

Elevated body mass index (BMI) predicts increases in body dissatisfaction in adolescents (Paxton et al., 2006), and the development of body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, and strategies to lose weight in pre-adolescent children (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2005; Saling et al., 2005). However, few studies have examined the role of body size in body image development in young children. One cross-sectional investigation reported a small correlation between BMI and body dissatisfaction in 5-year-old girls (Davison et al., 2000). Higher BMI has also been found to predict the desire for thinness in 5- to 8-year-old girls over a 12-month period (Dohnt & Tiggemann, 2006). However, in early childhood when cognitive abilities are underdeveloped, perceived body size may have a greater impact on body image than actual body size (Truby & Paxton, 2002). Research is needed to explore the influence of both actual BMI and perceived body size on body image in young girls and boys.

The aim of this study was to explore cross-sectional and prospective relationships between psychological characteristics and body size, and body image outcomes in children at ages 6 and 7. To date, no research has addressed this important question. Consistent with pre-adolescent and adolescent findings, we hypothesized that lower self-esteem, greater negative affect, perfectionism, internalization of appearance ideals, and larger actual and perceived body size would be cross-sectionally correlated with greater body dissatisfaction, and greater dietary restraint, drive for muscularity, and internalization of appearance ideals in children at ages 6 and 7 (Hypothesis 1). It was also hypothesized that lower self-esteem, greater negative affect, perfectionism, internalization, and larger perceived and actual body size at age 6 would predict greater body dissatisfaction, and greater dietary restraint, drive for muscularity, and internalization of appearance ideals at age 7 (Hypothesis 2).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited in Melbourne, Australia, as part of a longitudinal investigation, the Children’s Body Image Development Study, when children were 3 years old. Recruitment included advertisements in preschool and childcare centers and children’s entertainment settings. Data from children at ages 6 and 7 are reported in this paper.

At Time 1, participants were 235 children (girls, n = 129; boys, n = 106; Mage = 6.50 years, SD = 0.37), 219 of their mothers (Mage = 41.30 years, SD =

Participant characteristics

Table 1 presents descriptive data for children at Times 1 and 2. According to WHO (2011) BMI-for-age criteria, at Time 1, 76% of children were considered ‘normal’ weight, 3.9% underweight, 12% overweight, and 8.2% obese. At Time 2, 75% of children were classified ‘normal’ weight, 2.5% underweight, 14% overweight, and 8.5% obese. For BMI and perceived figure size, there was no significant main effect of time, Wilks’ λ = .98, F(2, 184) = 1.67, p = .191, partial η2 = .02, or gender, Wilks’

Discussion

Our investigation is the first to explore cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between psychological characteristics, body size, and body image attitudes and concerns in 6- and 7-year-old children. Findings revealed partial support for our hypotheses. A range of cross-sectional relationships was supported. Specifically, low self-esteem, high levels of perfectionism, and greater internalization of appearance ideals were associated with body dissatisfaction in children at ages 6 and 7.

Funding source

This research was funded by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project Grant (DP140101657) to S. Paxton and E. Wertheim.

Declaration of interests

None.

References (67)

  • P. Muris et al.

    The self-perception profile for children: Further evidence for its factor structure, reliability, and validity

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2003)
  • P. Muris et al.

    Biological, psychological, and sociocultural correlates of body change strategies and eating problems in adolescent boys and girls

    Eating Behaviors

    (2005)
  • S.J. Paxton et al.

    The development of body image and weight bias in childhood

    Advances in Child Development and Behavior

    (2017)
  • L.A. Ricciardelli et al.

    A biopsychosocial model for understanding body image and body change strategies among children

    Applied Developmental Psychology

    (2003)
  • L.H. Shriver et al.

    Child gender and weight status moderate the relation of maternal feeding practices to body esteem in 1st grade children

    Appetite

    (2015)
  • E.C. Spiel et al.

    Weight attitudes in 3 to 5-year-old children: Age differences and cross-sectional predictors

    Body Image

    (2012)
  • M. Tiggemann

    Body dissatisfaction and adolescent self-esteem: Prospective findings

    Body Image

    (2005)
  • E.H. Wertheim et al.

    Body image development – Adolescent girls

  • A.E. Wojtowicz et al.

    Weighing in on risk factors for body dissatisfaction: A one-year prospective study of middle-adolescent girls

    Body Image

    (2012)
  • T.M. Achenbach

    Manual for Child Behavior Checklist/ 4–18 and 1991 profile

    (1991)
  • Australian Bureau of Statistics

    Census of population and housing: Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA). (2033.0.55.001)

    (2011)
  • S.K. Bearman et al.

    The skinny on body dissatisfaction: A longitudinal study of adolescent girls and boys

    Journal of Youth and Adolescence

    (2006)
  • A.T. Beck

    Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders

    (1976)
  • C. Bento et al.

    Perfectionism and eating behaviour in Portuguese adolescents

    European Eating Disorders Review

    (2010)
  • L. Boone et al.

    Perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, and bulimic symptoms: The intervening role of perceived pressure to be thin and thin ideal internalization

    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (2011)
  • L. Boone et al.

    When or why does perfectionism translate into eating disorder pathology? A longitudinal examination of the moderating and mediating role of body dissatisfaction

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2014)
  • T.F. Cash

    Cognitive behavioural perspectives on body image

  • S.R. Damiano et al.

    Dietary restraint of 5-year-old girls: Associations with internalization of the thin ideal and maternal, media, and peer influences

    The International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (2015)
  • L. Dickie et al.

    What components of perfectionism predict drive for thinness?

    Eating Disorders

    (2012)
  • H. Dohnt et al.

    The contribution of peer and media influences to the development of body satisfaction and self-esteem in young girls: A prospective study

    Developmental Psychology

    (2006)
  • S.J. Durkin et al.

    An integrative model of the impact of exposure to idealised female images on adolescent girls’ body satisfaction

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (2007)
  • D.W. Evans et al.

    Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: The prevalence and development of compulsive-like behaviour in normal young children

    Child Development

    (1997)
  • F. Faul et al.

    Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses

    Behaviour Research Methods

    (2009)
  • Cited by (25)

    • Risk factors in body image dissatisfaction: gender, maladaptive perfectionism, and psychological wellbeing

      2022, Heliyon
      Citation Excerpt :

      Body image dissatisfaction often arises from an upward social comparison with negative self-appraisals leading to maladaptive behaviours to try to cope with the reactions (Chang et al., 2018). To correct perceived body image flaws, extreme including maladaptive behaviours may be adopted such as seen in the demand for cosmetic surgery reported in 2015 (American Society of Plastic Surgeons, 2016), and the increases in eating disorders and practices, excessive exercise, increased consumption of diuretics or laxatives, and induced vomiting (Goltz et al., 2013; Kenny and Hicks, 2014; Nichols et al., 2018; Juarascio et al., 2011; Stapleton et al., 2017). Some recent research has suggested that females experience higher levels of body image dissatisfaction than do males or the research has targeted females indicating that being female is a risk factor (e.g., Donaghue, 2008; Esnaola et al., 2010; Franchina and Coco, 2018; Jones et al., 2007).

    • Perfectionism as a vulnerability following appearance-focussed social comparison: A multi-wave study with female adolescents

      2022, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      According to Blatt (1995), perfectionism and self-criticism share core characteristics of unworthiness, self-conscious affect, and a fear of disapproval, which together yield vulnerability to depression and perceptions of interpersonal inferiority. Our findings substantiate these ideas by showing self-critical perfectionism is positively correlated with depression and body dissatisfaction (e.g., Nichols, Damiano, Gregg, Wertheim, & Paxton, 2018; Smith, Sherry, et al., 2016). Among female adolescents, these findings are salient given the high rates of depressive symptoms and body dissatisfaction reported (e.g., Bornioli, Lewis-Smith, Slater, & Bray, 2021).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text