Elsevier

Cognitive Development

Volume 58, April–June 2021, 101043
Cognitive Development

Children’s experience of economic inequality: How earning position influences prosocial behavior

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101043Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Four- to six-year-old children played games with puppets and were awarded points.

  • The point distribution was either unequal (Experiment 1) or equal (Experiment 2).

  • In both experiments, children were also 1) high earners or 2) low earners.

  • In high inequality only, high earning children gave an extra point to poor puppets.

  • However, low earning children gave the point to rich puppets.

Abstract

Across two experiments, children aged 4–6 years (N = 120) played a series of games with six puppets where each accrued tokens to exchange for stickers. Children were high or low earners, experiencing high inequality in Experiment 1 and low inequality in Experiment 2. Prosocial behavior towards the puppets assessed how children treat those who directly experience the economic environment, and children’s donation behavior towards a poor, unknown child measured whether earning status affects children’s broader generosity. When experiencing high economic inequality, children were more likely to give a spare token to a puppet who was in a contrasting position (e.g., poor children gave to rich puppets and rich children gave to poor), suggesting children will give to those who are least likely to threaten their own relative position. This study demonstrates for the first time how economic status can alter young children’s prosocial behavior when confronted with inequality.

Introduction

Economic inequality can greatly influence how we interact with others in our environment. A population’s level of economic inequality is defined by its how wealth is distributed; when there is high economic inequality, poor individuals co-exist with wealthier individuals whereas when inequality is low, all individuals have relatively equal amounts of wealth (Wilkinson & Pickett, 2009). In adults, high inequality has been linked to reduced prosocial behavior (Côté, House, & Willer, 2015; Cronin, Acheson, Hernández, & Sánchez, 2015; Nishi, Shirado, Rand, & Christakis, 2015; Sands, 2017) and greater competitive sentiments (Sánchez-Rodríguez, Willis, Jetten, & Rodríguez-Bailón, 2018). However, children’s reactions to inequality remain underexplored (Ruck, Mistry, & Flanagan, 2019). This is problematic given a child’s rearing environment can have dramatic effects on cognition and behavior later in adulthood (e.g., Kolb & Gibb, 2011; Rijlaarsdam et al., 2013). Moreover, children’s concern for fairness, i.e., judgments about whether rewards adequately reflect the effort or skill put in to a task (Kirkland, Jetten, & Nielsen, 2019), begins to develop in the preschool years (e.g., Baumard, Mascaro, & Chevallier, 2012; Blake & McAuliffe, 2011), suggesting the pervasive effects of inequality may impact children from early in development. The current study thus aimed to investigate the impact of inequality and earning position on young children’s prosocial tendencies.

Children begin showing a concern for fairness and inequality from around three years of age (Hamann, Warneken, Greenberg, & Tomasello, 2011). However, their behavior does not always align with their judgments (Rizzo & Killen, 2016) - known as the knowledge-behavior gap (Blake, McAuliffe, & Warneken, 2014; Smith, Blake, & Harris, 2013). For example, preschoolers will verbally judge meritocratic outcomes as fair but will still divide resources equally regardless of merit (although see Baumard et al., 2012). However, these studies, as with most others in the field, focus on inequality between two individuals with children being passive, third-party observers. This focus misaligns with children’s experiences of economic inequality in the real world; inequality exists between many and individuals are rarely passive observers but are part of the unequal or more equal world around them. Only one study to date has directly addressed these limitations by examining the role of environmental inequality on children’s prosocial behavior.

Kirkland, Jetten, and Nielsen (2020) exposed four- to five-year-old children to a series of games played with six other puppets. As children and puppets played the games, they accrued tokens which could be later swapped for stickers. Importantly, the number of tokens received was never clearly correlated with the effort or skill of the individual. Children experienced either high inequality (some puppets earned few tokens/stickers while others earned a lot) or low inequality (puppets earned similar amounts). Across both conditions, children received the same number of tokens (i.e., 14) and finished in the same relative position (i.e., fourth place of seven).

At the conclusion of the games, children received 14 stickers and were asked if they would like to donate some to an unknown, fictional child (Kirkland et al., 2020). The children exposed to a high inequality environment donated fewer stickers to a child in need compared to children exposed to low inequality. Additionally, children were asked to give out six extra tokens to the puppets. Regardless of condition, children gave the puppets one token each and this provides further evidence of an early preference for giving equally (Rizzo & Killen, 2016). The paradigm established by Kirkland et al. (2020) was successful in creating an effective economic environment. However, children were middle earners across both high and low inequality. It remains unclear how children would react if they were low or high earners, i.e., ostensibly poor or rich.

Research with adults suggests that one’s economic position has an impact on how inequality is experienced (e.g., Cheung & Lucas, 2016; Côté et al., 2015; Jetten, Mols, & Postmes, 2015; Jetten et al., 2017). Jetten et al. (2017) postulated that inequality affects both the wealthy and poor negatively, albeit for slightly different reasons. They hypothesized that inequality enhances feelings of being deprived in poorer individuals whereas higher income individuals experience greater perceived insecurity of their status, leading to increased competitive sentiments and reduced cooperative behavior. Moreover, Côté et al. (2015) found that, compared to low inequality, high inequality negatively affects donation behavior in high income but not low income individuals (although, see Schmukle, Korndörfer, & Egloff, 2019, for alternative findings). This suggests that these different experiences at lower and higher positions in a hierarchy do not affect prosociality in the same way. Past work has shown young children recognise differences in wealth, and use these cues to guide their behavior (Shutts, Brey, Dornbusch, Slywotzky, & Olson, 2016). However, it remains unclear if relative position impacts children’s experience of unequal and equal environments, and how this may alter prosocial behavior.

The current study aimed to identify the role of high and low earning positions on prosocial behaviour in environments with varying degrees of inequality. We examined this in four- to six-year-olds as prior research has established that preschoolers have a basic understanding of fairness (Hamann, Bender, & Tomasello, 2014; Rizzo & Killen, 2016; Smith et al., 2013). In addition, preschoolers are sensitive to unequal outcomes (Hamann et al., 2011) and are less prosocial in the face of inequality (Kirkland et al., 2020). Thus, in the current study, four- to six-year-old children played a game with six puppets. Across four games, children and puppets completed the task and were rewarded tokens to be later exchanged for stickers. Children were then provided with a donation task where they were asked to donate some of their stickers to a needy child, and a resource division task where they were given an extra token to give to a puppet. These tasks differed in terms of generalisability; the resource division task assesses how children treat others directly implicated by the economic environment whereas the donation task examines whether inequality may have broader implications for generosity toward strangers, as it appears to in adults (e.g., Côté et al., 2015).

In Experiment 1, children experienced highly unequal outcomes between the puppets, where some received a small amount and others received a large amount. In addition, children themselves were either low or high earners. In Experiment 2, to clarify the role of economic status in the absence of a broader unequal environment, children were again either low or high earners but instead experienced highly equal outcomes between the puppets. Notably, comparing the role of earning position (i.e., high or low) directly to inequality (i.e., high or low) affords insight into the interaction between inequality and economic status. However, such comparison creates an unavoidable confound; children’s position relative to others changes as the degree of inequality changes. That is, a high earner in a low inequality condition would possess more than the others by a significant degree, but a high earner in a high inequality condition would earn an amount similar to other high earners. Thus, we examined the role of high and low inequality in two separate experiments to mitigate this confound and maximise control.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

As mentioned, research in adults suggests that experiencing high inequality impacts general donation behavior in high income but not low income individuals (e.g., Côté et al., 2015). This suggests the negative effects of inequality for higher earners (because they experience greater status threat and competitiveness) are more pronounced than the negative effects of inequality on prosocial behavior for low earners (Jetten et al., 2017). Thus, in line with Côté et al. (2015), it was hypothesized

Experiment 2

The second experiment was designed to chart how being a high or low earner in a low inequality environment affects children’s prosocial behavior. Côté et al. (2015) reported that, compared to high inequality, low inequality in resources can lead to greater generosity from high income individuals compared to low income individuals. High earners have much more to give and have fewer feelings of threat regarding the stability of their position in more equal environments. Thus, it was hypothesized

Cross-experiment analysis

Based on advice from reviewers, a post-hoc decision was made to cross-analyse the results for the resource division task from Experiment 1 and 2. This decision was based on the results from the high earning children – a majority of children in the high inequality condition gave to the low earning puppet whereas those in low inequality predominantly gave to the high earning puppet. However, these results should be treated with caution. As discussed previously, comparing low and high inequality

General discussion

With wealth increasingly concentrated in the hands of a small elite, economic inequality has become a defining concern for the 21st century. For example, in the U.S.A., the level of income inequality has increased in the last two decades (The World Bank, 2019), and this has become a central issue in the race for the 2020 Democratic presidential nomination (Uhrmacher, 2020). It is thus integral to understand how exposure to such inequality might influence behavior, including that of young

Author contributions

Kelly Kirkland: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Project administration. Jolanda Jetten: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Supervision. Matti Wilks: Writing – review & editing, Supervision. Mark Nielsen: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Ethical approval

All methodological procedures used in the current study have been approved by an institutional review board, in accordance with the ethical standards dictated by the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.

Data availability statement

The current study was not formally preregistered as data collection was completed prior to our knowledge of the Open Science Framework (OSF). However, all raw and deidentified data is available on the OSF: https://osf.io/9438v/?view_only=0c6e2a6c954f4f5eb305d4dc6dc8a7be. All materials used in the current article are widely accessible.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Acknowledgements

We extend thanks to the Early Cognitive Development Centre (ECDC) at the University of Queensland, as well as all parents, children and ECDC staff members. We extend further thanks to Parima Sithi-Amnuai, Katie Lau and Yubo Zhang for assistance with data collection, coding and recruitment.

References (41)

  • F. Cheung et al.

    Income inequality is associated with stronger social comparison effects: The effect of relative income on life satisfaction

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2016)
  • S. Côté et al.

    High economic inequality leads higher-income individuals to be less generous

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

    (2015)
  • K.A. Cronin et al.

    Hierarchy is detrimental for human cooperation

    Scientific Reports

    (2015)
  • D. Fleming et al.

    Income inequality across Australian regions during the mining boom: 2001–11

    Australian Geographer

    (2015)
  • M. Green et al.

    The cost of helping: An exploration of compassionate responding in children

    British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (2018)
  • K. Hamann et al.

    Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in 3-year-olds

    Developmental Psychology

    (2014)
  • K. Hamann et al.

    Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees

    Nature

    (2011)
  • J. Henrich et al.

    The weirdest people in the world?

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2010)
  • J. Jetten et al.

    Relative deprivation and relative wealth enhances anti-immigrant sentiments: The V-curve re-examined

    PloS One

    (2015)
  • K. Kirkland et al.

    But that’s not fair! The experience of economic inequality from a child’s perspective

  • Cited by (6)

    • Children's thinking about group-based social hierarchies

      2022, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Better understanding the development of sociopolitical worldviews during childhood and the contexts under which children’s worldviews align with or diverge from those of their parents are crucial avenues for future inquiry. A fourth factor shaping whether and how children respond to group-based hierarchies is children’s own social standing (Box 4), with children from groups with more (vs. less) wealth, power, and status tending to be less likely to challenge group-based inequalities or social exclusion [110–115]. For example, there is evidence that White children evaluate interracial exclusion as less wrong than do Black children [110], and that children who self-identify as higher-wealth evaluate inter-wealth exclusion as less wrong than do children who self-identify as lower-wealth [113].

    • From “haves” to “have nots”: Developmental declines in subjective social status reflect children's growing consideration of what they do not have

      2022, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Children's early tendency not to engage in upward social comparison seems, at first glance, to contradict research on inequity aversion in childhood. Multiple studies have found that preschoolers do spontaneously notice when others receive less or more than them (Birch & Billman, 1986; Kirkland et al., 2021a; LoBue et al., 2011),will sometimes donate resources to low-resource peers (Kirkland et al., 2021b) or sacrifice resources to maintain equal resource distributions (Blake & McAuliffe, 2011; McAuliffe, Blake, Steinbeis, & Warneken, 2017). However, we believe that young children's aversion to being at a disadvantage, as demonstrated in inequity aversion research, may directly motivate children's neglect to consider what they do not have.

    View full text