Emergent literacy profiles among prekindergarten children from low-SES backgrounds: Longitudinal considerations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.03.007Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We identified fall and spring preschool emergent literacy profiles.

  • We examined changes in profile patterns and stability of membership.

  • Over half of children remained in similar profiles over time.

  • Children who made desirable shifts had higher initial language skills.

Abstract

This study represents a longitudinal follow-up to a prior investigation that identified five profiles of emergent literacy skills among children in the fall of their prekindergarten year. Here, we examined: (a) how profile patterns changed from fall to spring, (b) the extent to which children remained stable in their profile membership, and (c) possible factors that may explain children’s movement in profile membership from fall to spring. Participants were 484 children enrolled in needs-based programs. Eight measures of emergent literacy, across oral language and code-related domains, were administered in both the fall and the spring of the prekindergarten year. Latent profile analysis was used to classify children into fall and spring profiles (N = 484, 369, respectively). Although there were fewer profiles identified in the spring, children’s membership within the profiles was characterized by a degree of stability, especially in the extremes of performance. Among children who shifted membership from fall to spring (35%), movement to a more desirable profile was primarily associated with children’s emergent literacy skills, particularly oral language, at the beginning of the prekindergarten year.

Section snippets

Theoretical model of emergent literacy

Emergent literacy skill is a general term that describes specific competencies of young children that are precursors to future reading achievements in either word recognition or comprehension (National Early Literacy Panel [NELP], 2008). These foundational skills can be divided into two domains, or strands, namely, oral language skills and code-related skills (Storch and Whitehurst, 2002, Whitehurst and Lonigan, 1998, Whitehurst and Lonigan, 2001). Oral language skills include grammar and

The stability of children’s literacy skills

As used in the field of developmental science, stability generally refers to the progress children make in relation to their peers from one year to the next (i.e., relative rank; Wagner et al., 1997). The question of whether pre-k children’s emergent literacy skills exhibit stability over time is an important one, as research on reading achievement in the primary grades, when formal reading instruction begins, has clearly documented that children’s literacy skills and ranking against peers are

Aims of this study

This study builds upon a recent work describing profiles of emergent literacy skills across an array of oral language (i.e., receptive/expressive grammar and receptive/expressive vocabulary) and code-related skills (i.e., print concepts, alphabet knowledge, name writing, rhyme awareness) among 492 low-SES pre-k children in the fall of the academic year (Cabell et al., 2011a). Through cluster analysis techniques, five psychometrically sound profiles were empirically derived and ordered for

Participants

Participants were 484 children from 93 classrooms enrolled in two larger intervention studies of teaching practices within preschool classrooms (N = 665 children from 106 classrooms) in a single mid-Atlantic state. Of the children for whom parental consent was received, approximately six to eight were randomly selected from each classroom. Participants were enrolled in preschool programs serving children considered at-risk for later academic difficulty, with a majority of children from low-SES

Results

Overall, children exhibited a wide range of ability across all emergent literacy skills. Table 2 displays raw-score means and standard deviations across skills during both the fall and spring time-points. Children’s standard language scores at both fall and spring time-points fell below the mean of the more general population (i.e., CELF composite language scores were 29th, 36th percentiles, respectively). With regard to code-related skills, children recognized about 8 letters in the fall and

Discussion

The year prior to kindergarten entry is a period during which policymakers and researchers alike have focused attention as an optimal time for interventions aimed at reducing children’s risk for later academic difficulties. There is a particular emphasis on enhancing the emergent literacy skills of children from low-SES backgrounds who often enter kindergarten behind their more advantaged peers, and a closer look at children’s performance over time is needed to understand the meaningful

References (78)

  • M. Chatterji

    Reading achievement gaps, correlates, and moderators of early reading achievement: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS) kindergarten to first grade sample

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2006)
  • H.K. Craig et al.

    Recent research on the language and literacy skills of African American students in the early years

  • D.A. Crone et al.

    Age and schooling effects on emergent literacy and early reading skills

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1999)
  • Crosnoe, R., Morrison, F., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Keating, D., Friedman, S. L., et al., The Eunice Kennedy Shriver...
  • B.D. DeBaryshe et al.

    Experimental validation of a preschool emergent literacy curriculum

    Early Education and Development

    (2007)
  • Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children’s reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade (NCES...
  • D.K. Dickinson

    Teachers’ language practices and academic outcomes of preschool children

    Science

    (2011)
  • D.K. Dickinson et al.

    Speaking out for language: Why language is central to reading development

    Educational Researcher

    (2010)
  • D.K. Dickinson et al.

    The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2003)
  • D.T. Dodge et al.

    The creative curriculum for preschool

    (2002)
  • G.J. Duncan et al.

    Reducing poverty through preschool interventions

    The Future of Children

    (2007)
  • L.M. Dunn et al.

    Peabody picture vocabulary test—Third edition (PPVT-III)

    (1997)
  • Early Childhood Research Institute on Measuring Growth and Development. (1998). Research and development of individual...
  • L. Ehri et al.

    Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis

    Reading Research Quarterly

    (2001)
  • D.J. Francis et al.

    Developmental lag versus deficit model of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curve analysis

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1996)
  • D. Fuchs et al.

    Introduction to response to intervention: What, why, and how valid is it?

    Reading Research Quarterly

    (2006)
  • H.K. Gerde et al.

    Teacher education, book-reading practices, and children’s language growth across one year of Head Start

    Early Education and Development

    (2009)
  • L. Girolametto et al.

    Training day care staff to facilitate children’s language

    American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

    (2003)
  • B. Hart et al.

    Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children

    (1995)
  • E. Hoff

    The use of longitudinal data and person-centered analyses in the study of cognitive and language development

    Merrill-Palmer Quarterly

    (2006)
  • M. Hohman et al.

    Educating young children

    (1995)
  • B. Hohnen et al.

    The structure of genetic influences on general cognitive, language, phonological, and reading abilities

    Developmental Psychology

    (1999)
  • M.A. Invernizzi et al.

    Phonological awareness literacy screening for preschool: Teacher’s manual

    (2004)
  • C. Juel

    Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1988)
  • L.M. Justice et al.

    Measuring preschool attainment of print-concept knowledge: A study of typical and at-risk 3- to 5-year-old children using item response theory

    Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools

    (2006)
  • L.M. Justice et al.

    Emergent literacy intervention for vulnerable preschoolers: Relative effects of two approaches

    American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology

    (2003)
  • L.M. Justice et al.

    Accelerating preschoolers’ early literacy development through classroom-based teacher-child storybook reading and explicit print referencing

    Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools

    (2009)
  • L.M. Justice et al.

    Language and literacy curriculum supplement for preschoolers who are academically at-risk: A feasibility study

    Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools

    (2010)
  • L.M. Justice et al.

    Learning new words from storybooks: Findings from an intervention with at-risk kindergartners

    Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools

    (2005)
  • Cited by (56)

    • Language, analogical reasoning, and working memory skills in emergent literacy period: Typical and DLD trajectories via Cluster Analyses

      2022, Early Childhood Research Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, the authors found that the children who advanced to higher-performing profiles came from higher SES backgrounds. Regarding socioeconomic factors, there is also evidence on the impact of low SES background on emergent literacy skills that generate a pattern of poor performance, particularly when children (English-speaking preschoolers) come from families with low incomes (Cabell et al., 2013). Cabell et al. (2013) found relatively stable patterns of low oral language skills (receptive and expressive vocabulary, and receptive and expressive grammar), that were −1 to −1.5 SD of the mean, and broad code-related (word and PhA awareness) weaknesses between −0.5 and −1 SD of the mean in a sample associated to a need-based program.

    • Are Chinese urban children ready for preschool? Latent profiles and associated predictors

      2021, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      In a literature review on SES disadvantage, SES was significantly predictive of children's social competence, early language skills, and academic achievement (McLoyd, 1998). Maternal education level was predictive of profile classification in a group of Head Start children's school readiness (McWayne, Cheung, et al., 2012) and of their shifting between groups across time (Cabell et al., 2013; Halle et al., 2012). Also, family SES significantly predicted preschoolers' profile membership of literacy skills (Ren et al., 2019) and school readiness profile (Hair et al., 2006).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    We thank the many teachers, children, and research staff who made this study possible, with special mention to Amy Sofka, Alice Wiggins, Elizabeth Cottone, Khara Pence Turnbull, and Sarah Friel. This research project was supported by Grants R305G050057 and R305F050124 from the U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences. The content of this publication does not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the Institute of Education Sciences, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations imply endorsement by the U.S. Department of Education.

    View full text