The quality of toddler child care and cognitive skills at 24 months: Propensity score analysis results from the ECLS-B

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.09.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The quantity, type, and quality of child care are reported on for US toddlers in 2003.

  • Low income children were in lower quality care than non-low income children.

  • Propensity score adjustment is used to account for parental selection into child care.

  • Quality of child care is significantly associated with cognitive skills at 24-months.

  • Increasing quality of care for low income children critical to closing cognitive skills gap.

Abstract

Over half of the toddlers in the US experience routine nonparental care, but much less is known about early care than about preschool care. This study analyzed 2-year-old child care and child outcome data from the nationally representative ECLS-B sample of children born in 2001. At two-years of age, 51% of children experienced exclusive parental care, 18% relative care, 15% family child care, and 16% center care. More children in nonparental care were in medium quality care (61%) than in high quality (26%) or low quality (13%) care. Low-income children were more likely than non-low income children to be cared for by their parents and, when in care, were more often in lower quality care. The impact of toddler care quality on cognitive skills was estimated using propensity score adjustments to account for potential selection confounds due to family and child characteristics. Children's cognitive scores were higher in high or medium quality care than in low quality care, but no evidence emerged suggesting that poverty moderated the quality effects. Nevertheless, this suggests that increasing the proportion of low-income children in high quality care could reduce the achievement gap because low-income children are very unlikely to experience high quality care.

Introduction

The care of infants and toddlers has undergone significant changes over the last 40 years. In decades past, the vast majority of 12–24-month-old children were cared for at home by their mothers. By the mid 2000s, however, less than half of infants and toddlers in the US stayed at home with their mothers, indicating that the majority of 1- to 2-year-old US children receive part- or full-time care by people other than their parents (Laughlin, 2010). Ensuring that infants and toddlers benefit from positive early experiences is important to parents, and is of growing concern for policy makers. Since most child care research focuses on the preschool period, little is known about the impact of nonparental child care experiences during the infant and toddler period, especially at the national level. The purpose of this study is to extend our understanding of the impact of child care experiences for infants and toddlers by analyzing a nationally representative data source on children's early development, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth cohort of children born in 2001 (Flanagan & West, 2005). Propensity score methods are used to reduce bias in the estimation of the effect of child care quality on the cognitive development of 2-year-old children owing to parental selection of different types and quality of child care.

The quality of care received by infants and toddlers is thought to be crucial to their development, and in order to provide maximal developmental benefit, child–adult interactions must be continually sustained and grow increasingly complex over time (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). Increasing complexity arises when caregiver–child interactions are scaffolded to reflect the child's understanding of relationships and objects. Constructionist theorists extend the interaction-based child care quality model to argue that young children construct their own learning by interacting with and operating in their natural environments (Bodrova and Leong, 2006, Piaget, 2007, Vygotsky, 1978). These theories have been influential in shaping our understanding of the quality of child care, and specifically the importance of both the quality of adult–child interactions and the child's opportunity to interact with varied and rich materials within the child care classroom. In prior child care research, developmentally supportive environments were found to be rich in spoken language experiences; encourage children to safely explore their physical, social, and intellectual environment; and contain play environments that stimulate children's cognitive development (NICHD ECCRN, 2000a, Vandell, 2004). We define high quality child care as care that occurs in safe settings where children are provided with rich play environments and reciprocal interactions that encourage exploration and learning.

Studies of preschool children consistently find a positive association between child care quality and children's cognitive skills (Burchinal and Cryer, 2003, Burchinal et al., 2011, Camilli et al., 2010, Gormley et al., 2005Howes et al., 2008, Mashburn et al., 2008, NICHD ECCRN and Duncan, 2003, NICHD ECCRN, 2000a, NICHD ECCRN, 2002, NICHD ECCRN, 2005, Peisner-Feinberg and Burchinal, 1997, Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2001, Pianta et al., 2009, Reynolds et al., 2002Vandell, 2004, Votruba-Drzal et al., 2004). However, much less is known about child care quality effects on infant and toddler cognitive development. Experimental studies, which eliminate bias due to parental selection into different types of child care, find that, at 36 months, children's cognitive skills are enhanced by high-quality child care, with effect sizes ranging from d = .12 in the Early Head Start Study (Love et al., 2005) to d = .83 in the Infant Health and Development Program (McCormick et al., 2006) to d = 1.23 in the Abecedarian study (Campbell, Pungello, Miller-Johnson, Burchinal, & Ramey, 2001). The quality of care in these interventions was likely to be high due to involvement of research staff and use of evidence-based curricula, but child care quality was not actually measured in the treatment or control conditions in the Abecedarian and Infant Health and Development Program interventions.

In the case of younger children (12–24 months), the focus of the present paper, associations between child care quality and cognitive skills are rarely investigated, and were observed in a handful of non-experimental studies. These include both small, single-site studies (Burchinal, Roberts, Nabors, & Bryant, 1996) and larger multisite studies using data collected in the 1990s (NICHD ECCRN, 2006, NICHD ECCRN and Duncan, 2003). Child samples in prior work were primarily low income (e.g., Early Head Start) or from middle-class backgrounds (e.g., National Institute of Child Health and Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development [NICHD-SECCYD]) and thus not fully representative. While studies using the National Longitudinal Study of Youth (Han, Waldfogel, & Brooks-Gunn, 2001) are nationally representative, they do not adequately address associations between child care quality and cognitive skills due to the fact that child care quality was not measured. In addition, few large observational studies use statistically rigorous methods to account for parental selection into differing levels of child care quality, types of care, and quantity of care (an exception is work by Dearing, McCartney, and Taylor (2009) with the NICHD-SECCYD sample). The present study therefore uses propensity scores to examine the effect of early child care on cognitive skills at 24 months for children in the ECLS-B, a nationally representative sample of children born in 2001.

In order to account for the relation between child care quality and cognitive skills in an unbiased manner, it is necessary to also account for other facets of early child care that, in prior work, have been linked to children's cognitive skills. Principal among these are the amount of time children spend in child care, the age at which they enter care, and the type of care they receive. Of these, the type of care a child receives (center care or not) may be particularly important because being in center care is associated with increases in cognitive skills (Clarke-Stewart, Gruber, & Fitzgerald, 1994). While center care predicts cognitive skills over and above the measured quality of care in many studies (Brooks-Gunn et al., 2002, NICHD ECCRN and Duncan, 2003, NICHD ECCRN, 2000b, NICHD ECCRN, 2006), other research finds no cognitive benefit (measured at age 3) for center care vs. mother-only care during the first year of a child's life (Han et al., 2001). Loeb, Bridges, Bassok, Fuller, and Rumberger (2007) found that first entering center care between ages 2 and 3 resulted in greater cognitive gains than entering center care in the year prior to kindergarten. Beginning center care prior to two years of age yielded no additional cognitive benefits. Studies that simultaneously examined the amount of time children spent in child care and the quality of child care found only a quality effect on cognitive outcomes (NICHD ECCRN, 2000b, NICHD ECCRN, 2006, NICHD ECCRN and Duncan, 2003).

Given the above evidence, it is questionable how important these other dimensions of child care are in terms of predicting cognitive skills over and above the measured quality of child care. However, these dimensions of child care may be linked with cognitive skills, and cannot be ignored since doing so may bias any estimates of the effects of child care quality. The present study therefore tests whether these dimensions are related to cognitive skills.

Whether or not child care quality has an overall impact on cognitive skills, it is important to test for the effect of quality on low-income children, since public policies are often targeted toward children from low-income backgrounds. Additionally, estimating the relative magnitudes of effect for low-income and middle-income children is useful for understanding the ability of subsidized, high-quality child care for low-income children to narrow achievement gaps. Some evidence from the preschool period suggests that high-quality care might be a protective factor for low-income children (Burchinal, Roberts, Zeisel, Hennon, & Hooper, 2006). Quality of care appears to be related to cognitive development for all children (Vandell, 2004), but may be a stronger predictor for low-income children in the first year of grade school (Dearing et al., 2009). Although Dearing et al. (2009) considered early care, they did not isolate it from later child care, instead measuring child care quality by the number of times a child experienced high-quality care from 6 to 54 months. It is, thus, important to know whether high-quality infant/toddler care is protective separate from high-quality preschool care.

This study has two aims. First, descriptive information about toddler care experiences are provided using the nationally representative Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey-Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) to address the need for more information about the type, quantity, and especially quality of child care for toddlers in the US. Second, it addresses the understudied question of whether early cognitive development is related to the quality of toddler care. The ECLS-B provides rich information on children's socio-demographic and psychological characteristics that enable us to use propensity score procedures to reduce bias in estimating the relation between measured child care quality and children's cognitive development from 9 to 24 months of age. We hypothesize that higher quality child care promotes toddlers’ cognitive development.

Section snippets

Participants

Data are drawn from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort, a nationally representative, longitudinal cohort study of US children born in 2001 (Flanagan & West, 2005). Sampling from birth certificate records, the ECLS-B oversampled Asian and Pacific Islanders, Native Americans and Alaska Natives, low-birth-weight (1500–2500 g) and very-low-birth-weight (less than 1500 g) children, as well as twins. At 9 and 24 months, children and their caregivers were assessed. The ECLS-B collected

Child care experiences for 24-month-old toddlers

The first set of analyses described the child care experiences at 2 years for the nationally representative sample, ECLS-B. Table 2 provides nationally representative estimates of the type, quantity, and quality of care as experienced by children who were 24 months of age in 2003, both for the entire sample and separately by poverty status.

Discussion

In this paper, we employed a nationally representative sample to examine the distribution of the quality of toddler care and its relation to cognitive performance at 24 months of age. As with prior work with preschool-age children, the results demonstrate that nonparent care is now normative for a large fraction of US toddlers, and that child care quality is related to early cognitive development as well as to preschool language, academic, and social development.

The descriptive analyses of the

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the Institute of Education Sciences (IES) for supporting this work through grant R305A090467-10 awarded to Principal Investigator George Farkas and Co-Principal Investigators Margaret Burchinal and Greg J. Duncan. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute or the US Department of Education. Research reported in this publication was also supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human

References (49)

  • M. Burchinal et al.

    How well do our measures of quality predict child outcomes? A meta-analysis and coordinated analysis of data from large-scale studies of early childhood settings

  • M.R. Burchinal et al.

    Quality of center child care and infant cognitive and language development

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • M. Burchinal et al.

    Social risk and protective child, parenting, and child care factors in early elementary school years

    Parenting: Science and Practice

    (2006)
  • G. Camilli et al.

    Meta-analysis of the effects of early education interventions on cognitive and social development

    Teachers College Record

    (2010)
  • F.A. Campbell et al.

    The development of cognitive and academic abilities: Growth curves from an early intervention educational experiment

    Developmental Psychology

    (2001)
  • A. Clarke-Stewart et al.

    Children at home and in day care

    (1994)
  • E. Dearing et al.

    Does higher quality early child care promote low-income children's math and reading achievement in middle childhood?

    Child Development

    (2009)
  • K.D. Flanagan et al.

    Children born in 2001: First results from the base year of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B)

    Education Statistics Quarterly

    (2005)
  • W.C. Gamble et al.

    Parental perceptions of characteristics of nonparental child care: Belief dimensions, family, and child correlates

    Journal of Child and Family Studies

    (2009)
  • W.T. Gormley et al.

    The effects of universal pre-K on cognitive development

    Developmental Psychology

    (2005)
  • W. Han et al.

    The effects of early maternal employment on later cognitive and behavioral outcomes

    Journal of Marriage and Family

    (2001)
  • T. Harms et al.

    Infant/toddler Environment Rating Scale

    (1990)
  • (1995)
  • S.W. Helburn et al.

    Child care cost and quality

    The Future of Children

    (1996)
  • Cited by (66)

    • Do child care characteristics during toddlerhood explain income-based gaps in reading and math skills at preschool?

      2021, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, we examined differences in the ECE characteristics between children from low-, middle-, and high-income families. We hypothesized that there would be income differences between each of these groups on these characteristics, with the most notable differences being between children from low- and high-income families, based upon prior literature (Coley et al., 2014; Dowsett et al., 2008; Fuller et al., 2020; Phillips et al., 1994; Ruzek et al., 2014). Second, we examined whether the ECE characteristics at age 2 predicted children’s reading and math skills at age 4.

    • Multi-tiered system of supports in early childhood: identifying gaps, considerations for application, and solutions

      2021, Early Childhood Research Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, language has a predictive and/or reciprocal relation with behavior as evidenced in ECE social-emotional interventions that include aspects of receptive and expressive language skills (e.g., identifying emotions; using words to express needs and feelings) and functional communication (e.g., Tier 3 behavior intervention plans; Girard, Pingault, Doyle, Falissard, & Tremblay, 2016; Helland, Lundervold, Heimann and Posserud, 2014; Monopoli & Kingston, 2012; Petersen, Bates, & Staples, 2015). In elementary schools, it is possible to treat literacy, math, and behavior separately from language skills, but the integration of language for the promotion of the other domains in ECE is readily accepted and strategically addressed in high quality ECE (Auger, Farkas, Burchinal, Duncan, & Vandell, 2014; Ruzek, Burchinal, Farkas, & Duncan, 2014). In fact, if oral language is neglected, progress in later reading may be hampered (Sinatra, Zygouris-Coe, & Dasinger, 2012).

    • A multilevel approach to ECEC policies and intensity of formal childcare participation of young children in Europe

      2021, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Numerous studies focussed on preschool-age children and the research evidence on the positive effects of childcare for 3–6 years old children upon development are fairly consistent (see Melhuish et al., 2015). Nevertheless, the findings on the relationships between attendance or amount of early childcare for 0–2-year-olds and children’s development are mixed: negative effects, no effects and positive effects have been demonstrated (Felfe & Lalive, 2018; Ruzek, Burchinal, Farkas, & Duncan, 2014; Zaslow et al., 2010). Even with the unequivocal findings, early childcare participation of 0–2 years-old children is still understudied.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text