Elsevier

Journal of School Psychology

Volume 65, December 2017, Pages 69-82
Journal of School Psychology

Latent national subpopulations of early education classroom disengagement of children from underresourced families

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2017.07.002Get rights and content

Abstract

This research examined the latent developmental patterns for early classroom disengagement among children from some of the most underresourced families in the nation. Based on standardized teacher observations from the Head Start Impact Study, a nationally representative sample of children (N = 1377) was assessed for manifestations of reticent/withdrawn and low energy behavior over four years spanning prekindergarten through first grade. For each form of disengagement, latent growth mixture modeling revealed three distinct subpopulations of change patterns featuring a dominant class associated with generally good classroom adjustment, a medial class that varied close to the population average over time, and a more extreme class (about 10% of the population) whose adjustment was relatively marginal and sometimes reached problematic levels. Whereas reticent/withdrawn behavior ordinarily subsided over time, low energy behavior increased. More extreme low energy behaviors tended to dissipate through schooling and extreme reticence/withdrawal became more accentuated, with both types associated with later academic and social problems. Attendant risk and protective factors are identified and mitigating assessment and prevention measures are discussed.

Introduction

Successful early schooling is dependent on the positive participation of children with teachers, classmates, and the learning activities that surround them. This positive participation is often called classroom engagement and the interactions that flow from it are deemed formative for eventual cognitive and social-emotional development (Mashburn et al., 2008, McWilliam and Casey, 2008, McWilliam et al., 2003, Willford et al., 2013) because they show the child how to encounter novelty and risk, seek assistance where needed, master new learning challenges, interact appropriately with teachers and peers, and respond reflectively and resiliently to failure and opposition (Fredricks et al., 2004, McDermott et al., 2011).

The nature of classroom engagement and the relative structure of the classroom environs change as children transition from preschool to school (Fredricks et al., 2004, Powell et al., 2008, Searle et al., 2014), but the residual effects of these early experiences have lasting influence for subsequent encounters with formal learning and life in general. Ample research literature supports that the quality of early school engagement connects predicatively to later educational achievement (Chien et al., 2010, Fredricks et al., 2004, Greenwood et al., 2002), to social and emotional outcomes (McDermott, Rikoon, & Fantuzzo, 2016), and to school retention, absenteeism, delayed graduation, and accelerated dropout (Alexander et al., 1997, Buhs and Ladd, 2001, Ladd et al., 1999, Searle et al., 2014).

Although classroom engagement is always active, it is not always positive. Negative classroom engagement is illustrated by aggressive, hyperactive, impulsive, or defiant behavior; commonly referred to as externalizing or overactive behavior problems (Noone-Lutz et al., 2002, Willford et al., 2013). Alternatively, when children can't or won't engage either positively or negatively, the behavioral state is more properly termed disengagement. Disengagement is marked by behavioral detachment or disconnection that could be described as pervasive shyness, diffidence, unassertiveness, reticence, withdrawal, inattention, sluggishness, or chronic lethargy. As is true for active engagement, classroom disengagement also foreshadows distal outcomes, but they are almost uniformly undesirable, including academic nonproficiency and social-emotional disturbances (Barghaus et al., 2017, Drogalis et al., 2017, Fantuzzo et al., 2003, Fantuzzo et al., 2007, Noone-Lutz et al., 2002). The current study focused selectively on the kinds of classroom disengagement that are manifest among that segment of the early education population that is at relative greatest risk for subsequent undesirable outcomes―children from some of the nation's most economically underresourced households (per the Head Start Impact Study [HSIS];U.S. Department of Health and Human Services [DHHS], 2010). Specifically, the investigation followed the developmental transitions in the forms of disengagement that have established construct validity as well as continuity from the earliest year of prekindergarten through first grade. These forms of classroom disengagement are known as low energy behavior and reticence/withdrawal (McDermott, Watkins, Rovine, & Rikoon, 2013).

Low energy and reticent/withdrawn forms of classroom disengagement were first identified in a study of a large sample of Head Start children from the nation's 5th largest and most economically impoverished city (Noone-Lutz et al., 2002). These dimensions correlated below or near zero with all identified forms of negative classroom engagement (aggression, oppositionality, hyperactivity) but produced very strong positive multivariate associations with social disconnection during classroom play, as assessed through the Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale (Fantuzzo, Coolahan, Mendez, McDermott, & Sutton-Smith, 1998). Similar strong associations were observed with the classroom Inattention scale of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (Conners, 1990). In an extension of this work, Fantuzzo et al. (2003) jointly referred to low energy and reticence/withdrawal as forms of Head Start classroom disengagement and showed their strong inverse relationships to teacher assessments of children's physical movement and coordination and propensity to engage in social contexts with classmates and teachers. Based on a still larger and independent sample of Head Start children from the same urban locale, Fantuzzo et al. (2007) later confirmed the strong positive association between classroom disengagement and disconnected social interactions during play periods and also showed that the reticent form of disengagement is more likely to manifest in situations with teachers than with classmates.

The specific behaviors that define low energy and reticence/withdrawal were incorporated as items in the Adjustment Scales for Early Transition in Schooling (ASETS; McDermott et al., 2013). ASETS was chosen by the Head Start Bureau and DHHS (2010) to represent the social-emotional domain of child functioning within the national HSIS. The HSIS drew a large nationwide random sample of children who were eligible for Head Start entry and randomly permitted a portion to enter Head Start and the remaining portion to enter comparable non-Head Start preschool settings. Thus, the sample was designed to proportionately represent both Head Start enrollees and enrollees in alternative non-Head Start preschools. It further assessed participant children (both initial Head Start and non-Head Start enrollees) through two years of prekindergarten, kindergarten, and first grade, each year applying ASETS toward the close of the spring semester.

McDermott et al. (2013) used exploratory and confirmatory longitudinal factor analyses to establish the structural integrity and continuity of the ASETS low energy and reticence/withdrawal measures across time and, through IRT vertical equating and Bayesian scoring, established reliable scales that would reflect changes in performance while holding stable the integrity of the underlying disengagement constructs. The researchers demonstrated through multilevel individual growth-curve modeling that the low energy and Reticence/Withdrawal scales were sensitive to both linear change and to higher-order quadratic and cubic change. Moreover, they established that it was possible to isolate distinctly different change patterns as associated with children known to have later succeeded versus failed academically and to have appeared adjusted versus maladjusted by the close of first grade. Further, across the four years of HSIS, low energy and Reticence/Withdrawal assessments were significantly related to direct assessments of academic functioning, to teacher assessments of academic functioning, and to independent teacher and parent assessments of classroom and home social and emotional adjustment.

Whereas the prior work is fundamental and supportive of the prospect that low energy and reticence/withdrawal measures detect even complex curvilinear change over the early education years and furthermore can discover in retrospect the unique change patterns associated with children who eventually manifest desirable versus undesirable outcomes, it is unknown whether the measures can identify important latent subpopulations of children (developmental trajectories) that distinguish themselves in terms of growth patterns and levels of adjustment. It is also not known whether such distinct developmental trajectories are more or less likely related to eventual academic and social outcomes. Nor is there an understanding as to how such latent subpopulations of children might be unique in terms of antecedent child and family explanatory factors.

From a practical standpoint, school psychologists and other educators who are provided early warning of a child's likely membership in a given latent growth subpopulation and the prospects for more distant outcomes and possible explanatory factors are likely to be in a much better position when considering interventions or the need for them. In response, the current investigation was designed to identify latent subpopulations of each form of disengagement, how these subpopulations transition over the early school years, and how they relate to important outcomes and to precursor child and family characteristics.

It is evident from the foregoing research that some communities of children are far more vulnerable than others as it pertains to potential educational disengagement. Most affected are those who arrive in prekindergarten from economically underresourced homes and socially stressed communities (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2010, Isenberg et al., 2016). The challenges facing such children are daunting from the outset, considering that the average Head Start enrollee is functioning at the 15th to 20th percentile in areas of literacy, language, and mathematics (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2003, Kopack Klein et al., 2013). Further, these children, partly related to a dearth of strong social support networks (related to impoverishment, large proportions of new immigrants, English-language limitations, etc.), face high risk for continued academic and socioemotional stress (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2010, Isenberg et al., 2016). The discovery that the dominant forms of classroom disengagement among Head Start children are low energy and reticent/withdrawn behavior and confirmation that these same forms of disturbance maintain construct stability, measurement reliability, and sensitivity to simple and complex change throughout the early education period, offer substantial promise for understanding the mechanisms that yield different transition pathways, precursors, and outcomes for children at relative risk.

Within this context, we address three questions. First, are there latent and longitudinal subpopulations (developmental trajectories) of emergent low energy and reticent/withdrawn behavior as children transition through prekindergarten, kindergarten, and first grade? Second, do these subpopulations forecast scholastic nonproficiency and classroom adjustment problems (distal outcomes) at the close of prekindergarten and first grade? Third, to what extent do child personal and familial factors (gender, ethnicity, primary language, special needs, family structure, maternal age at childbirth, education, immigration) serve to affect the risk of child membership in the most problematic subpopulations of emergent classroom disengagement? Whereas prior research has highlighted the predictive agency of early classroom disengagement (Barghaus et al., 2017, Fantuzzo et al., 2003, Fantuzzo et al., 2007, Noone-Lutz et al., 2002), it has never done so by capturing a large and nationally representative probability sample that tracks disengagement throughout the early education years, nor has research identified important variations of developmental change, their related distal outcomes, and potentially explanatory precursors.

Section snippets

Participants

The national Head Start Impact Study (HSIS; DHHS, 2010) was a randomized control trial designed to measure the relative effectiveness of Head Start and comparable prekindergarten programs. Our study centers exclusively on that portion of the sample deemed the 3-year-old cohort because only that cohort was tracked through two years of prekindergarten and then kindergarten and first grade. Participants were drawn at random from 223 prekindergarten agencies across all geographic regions of the

Latent growth models

The longitudinal variable observations were nested within teachers who may have observed more than one child. Latent growth mixture modeling provides no practical mechanism for estimating parameters that are nested longitudinally. Rather, we calculated the harmonic mean number of children assessed by teachers for each dependent variable during each year and the associated design effect (Snijders, 2005). The grand harmonic mean number of children assessed per teacher = 1.13 (range 1.05–1.24),

Discussion

This research sought to discover the latent developmental patterns for early classroom disengagement among children from the most underresourced families in the nation. The focus was on low energy and reticent/withdrawn behavior because those are the specific subtypes found nationwide in early education; that is, the subtypes of disengagement characteristic of broad community rather than clinical samples. McDermott et al. (2013) showed that both forms of disengagement were capable of detecting

Conclusion

Children's earliest transitions in schooling are formative, requiring reciprocal learning incentives and child participation. We have for the case of those children viewed at greatest risk, mapped out both the common developmental pathways of classroom disengagement and the rarer pathways that tend to signal undesirable and perhaps adverse outcomes. For any such children the most impactful deterrent is sustained and artful promotion of positive classroom engagement. One approach is to structure

References (73)

  • D.H. Arnold et al.

    Parent involvement in preschool: Predictors and the relation of involvement to preliteracy development

    School Psychology Review

    (2008)
  • T. Asparouhov et al.

    Approaches for auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: 3-step approaches using Mplus (ver. 8)

  • K. Barghaus et al.

    Problems in classroom engagement: Validation of an assessment for district-wide use in the early primary grades

    Early Education and Development

    (2017)
  • M.H. Bornstein et al.

    Developmental pathways among adaptive functioning and externalizing and internalizing behavioral problems: Cascades from childhood into adolescence

    Applied Developmental Science

    (2013)
  • U. Bronfenbrenner et al.

    The ecology of developmental processes

  • E.S. Buhs et al.

    Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children's school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes

    Developmental Psychology

    (2001)
  • M.J. Bundick et al.

    Prompting student engagement in the classroom

    Teachers College Record

    (2014)
  • N.C. Chien et al.

    Children's classroom engagement and school readiness gains in prekindergarten

    Child Development

    (2010)
  • C.K. Conners

    Conners rating scales manual

    (1990)
  • A.R. Drogalis et al.

    Parent and teacher perspectives on psychological adjustment: A national measurement study in Trinidad and Tobago

    International Journal of School and Educational Psychology

    (2017)
  • R. Dumont et al.

    Test descriptions and reviews

  • T.E. Duncan et al.

    An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications

    (2006)
  • J. Fantuzzo et al.

    A multivariate analysis of emotional and behavioral adjustment and preschool educational outcomes

    School Psychology Review

    (2003)
  • J.F. Fantuzzo et al.

    Investigation of dimensions of social-emotional classroom behavior and school readiness for low-income urban preschool children

    School Psychology Review

    (2007)
  • J.W. Fantuzzo et al.

    An integrated curriculum to improve mathematics, language, and literacy for head start children

    American Educational Research Journal

    (2011)
  • Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics

    America's children in brief: Key national indicators of well-being, 2008

    (2008)
  • J.L. Fleiss et al.

    Statistical methods for rates and proportions

    (2003)
  • J.A. Fredricks et al.

    School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence

    Review of Educational Research

    (2004)
  • P.E. Greenbaum et al.

    Variation in the drinking trajectories of freshman college students

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2005)
  • C.R. Greenwood et al.

    Academic engagement: Current perspectives on research and practice

    School Psychology Review

    (2002)
  • K.S. Gross et al.

    Use of city archival data to inform dimensional structure of neighborhoods

    Journal of Urban Health

    (2008)
  • K.C. Herman et al.

    The impact of the incredible years parent, child, and teacher training programs on children's co-occurring internalizing symptoms

    School Psychology Quarterly

    (2011)
  • D.W. Hosmer et al.

    Applied logistic regression

    (2000)
  • A.C. Huston et al.

    Human development in social context

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2010)
  • E. Isenberg et al.

    Do low-income students have equal access to effective teachers? Evidence from 26 districts (Report No. NCEE 2017-4007)

  • A. Kopack Klein et al.

    Data tables for FACES 2009 report getting ready for kindergarten: Children's progress during Head Start (OPRE Report 2013-21b)

    (2013)
  • This research was conducted with the cooperation of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paul A. McDermott, Graduate School of Education, University of Pennsylvania, 3700 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6216.

    View full text