Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 70, October 2018, Pages 170-179
Neurobiology of Aging

Regular article
Estimates of age-related memory decline are inflated by unrecognized Alzheimer's disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Cognitive decline is considered an inevitable consequence of aging; however, estimates of cognitive aging may be influenced negatively by undetected preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD). This study aimed to determine the extent to which estimates of cognitive aging were biased by preclinical AD. Cognitively normal older adults (n = 494) with amyloid-β status determined from positron emission tomography neuroimaging underwent serial neuropsychological assessment at 18-month intervals over 72 months. Estimates of the effects of age on verbal memory, working memory, executive function, and processing speed were derived using linear mixed models. The presence of preclinical AD and clinical progression to mild cognitive impairment or dementia during the study were then added to these models as covariates. Initially, age was associated with decline across all 4 cognitive domains. With the effects of elevated amyloid-β and clinical progression controlled, age was no longer associated with decline in verbal or working memory. However, the magnitude of decline was reduced only slightly for executive function and was unchanged for processing speed. Thus, considered together, the results of the study indicate that undetected preclinical AD negatively biases estimates of age-related cognitive decline for verbal and working memory.

Introduction

Cognitive aging is a lifespan concept that describes changes in cognition that occur as individuals progress from young to very old age. In healthy older adults (i.e., >60 years old), cognitive aging is reported to involve decline across multiple domains of cognition, including episodic memory, working memory, executive function, and processing speed (Ritchie et al., 2016, Salthouse, 2010, Schaie, 2005, Wilson et al., 2015). However, emerging evidence suggests that at least some age-related cognitive decline may actually reflect the influence of biological processes, such as neurodegenerative disease, that are common in older adults but which cannot be considered to be normal aging (DeCarlo et al., 2014, Hofer and Sliwinski, 2001, Spiro and Brady, 2011). A challenge for models of cognitive aging is therefore to determine the nature and extent to which such processes may affect estimates of age-related cognitive decline.

It is now recognized that a high proportion of adults aged over 60 years, who show no impairment in cognition, mood, or behavior, harbor signs of Alzheimer's disease (AD) neuropathological changes, such as abnormal accumulation of amyloid-β (Aβ+), and as such can be considered to be in the preclinical stage of the disease (Sperling et al., 2011). For example, postmortem studies show that approximately 60%–98% of older adults aged over 60 years have evidence of AD neuropathological changes at death, despite having had no signs of cognitive impairment during life (Bennett et al., 2012, Monsell et al., 2014). Results from in vivo studies using cerebrospinal fluid and neuroimaging biomarkers also show that Aβ+ occurs in approximately 30% of cognitively normal older adults (Jack et al., 2014, Jansen et al., 2015, Vos et al., 2013). Importantly, for models of cognitive aging, the proportion of cognitively normal older adults with Aβ+ increases with age, from 10% at age of 50 years to 44% at age of 90 years (Jansen et al., 2015). As such, the potential for Aβ+ to influence estimates of cognitive aging also increases with age.

Given the relationship between the prevalence of preclinical AD and age, it is possible that psychological studies of cognitive aging have included individuals with preclinical AD in their samples of older adults. Review of the inclusion criteria of large studies of cognitive aging indicates that most aging studies exclude individuals with a clinical diagnosis of dementia from enrollment, and many also use brief cognitive screening tools such as the Mini-Mental State Examination or the Montreal Cognitive Assessment to exclude individuals with cognitive impairment from their samples (Harrington et al., 2017b). However, while such tests will identify individuals with dementia, they are insensitive to the mild cognitive difficulties that characterize the preclinical phase of the disease (Lacy et al., 2015, Spencer et al., 2013). For example, a recent meta-analysis demonstrated that Aβ+ in cognitively normal older adults is associated with a subtle decline (Cohen's d = −0.24 to −0.30) in global cognition, visuospatial function, processing speed, executive function, and episodic memory (Baker et al., 2016). In addition, few aging studies have assessed whether any of their participants progress to dementia during or at some short time after the study period, an event indicating that a neurodegenerative disease process was present during any earlier cognitive assessment (Sliwinski et al., 1996). Consequently, it is possible that the purportedly healthy samples of older adults from cognitive aging studies to date have included a substantial proportion of individuals with preclinical AD, and therefore, the effects of age on cognition may have been overestimated.

The aim of this study was to determine the influence of preclinical AD and progression to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia on estimates of age-related cognitive decline in a sample of cognitively normal older adults who have undergone objective assessment of physical and cognitive health before entry into the study. The first hypothesis was that older age would be associated with decline across all areas of cognition when Aβ+ and progression were not controlled. The second hypothesis was that the effect of age on cognition would be reduced when the effects of Aβ+ and progression were taken into account.

Section snippets

Participants

The study sample (n = 494) was drawn from the Australian Imaging Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study. At the baseline assessment, all participants were classified as cognitively normal, scored >24 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975), reported no history of stroke or transient ischemic attack, and were aged between 60 and 85 years (see Supplementary Figs. A and B for distribution of ages). All participants had undergone positron emission tomography (PET) neuroimaging

Sample characteristics

Table 2 summarizes the demographic and clinical characteristics for the sample. There were slightly more females than males; most of the sample had achieved tertiary levels of education; levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms were low; vital signs were on average within normal ranges; and the dementia risk score indicated that most of the sample had low-to-moderate relative risk of developing dementia. The Aβ+ group were older, more likely to carry an APOE ε4 allele, had higher dementia risk

Discussion

The first hypothesis, that age would be associated with decline across all areas of cognition when Aβ+ and progression were not controlled, was supported. In this prospective study of cognitively normal older adults, increasing age was associated with decline in verbal memory, executive function, processing speed, and working memory. When considered over 30 years, the magnitude of age-related cognitive decline observed was substantial, ranging from approximately 0.30 SD units for verbal memory

Disclosure statement

PM and AS are employees of Cogstate Ltd; YYL serves as a scientific consultant for Cogstate Ltd, Biogen, and Lundbeck. DA has served on scientific advisory boards for Novartis, Eli Lilly, Janssen, and Pfizer Inc. CLM is an advisor to Prana Biotechnology Ltd and a consultant to Eli Lilly.

Acknowledgements

Alzheimer's Australia (Victoria and Western Australia) assisted with promotion of the AIBL study and the screening of telephone calls from volunteers. The AIBL team wishes to thank the clinicians who referred patients with AD to the study: Associate Professor Brian Chambers, Professor Edmond Chiu, Dr Roger Clarnette, Associate Professor David Darby, Dr Mary Davison, Dr John Drago, Dr Peter Drysdale, Dr Jacqueline Gilbert, Dr Kwang Lim, Professor Nicola Lautenschlager, Dr Dina LoGiudice, Dr

References (85)

  • C. Jack et al.

    Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2010)
  • C. Jack et al.

    Age-specific population frequencies of amyloidosis and neurodegeneration among cognitively normal people age 50-89 years: a cross-sectional study

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2014)
  • W. Jagust

    Vulnerable neural systems and the borderland of brain aging and neurodegeneration

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • M. Kivipelto et al.

    Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged people: a longitudinal, population-based study

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2006)
  • S.L. Leal et al.

    Review: neurocognitive aging and the hippocampus across species

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2015)
  • S.J. Ritchie et al.

    Predictors of ageing-related decline across multiple cognitive functions

    Intelligence

    (2016)
  • C.C. Rowe et al.

    Amyloid imaging results from the Australian imaging, biomarkers and Lifestyle (aibl) study of aging

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2010)
  • Y.L. Shing et al.

    Episodic memory across the lifespan: the contributions of associative and strategic components

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2010)
  • R.A. Sperling et al.

    Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on aging and the Alzheimer's association workgroup

    Alzheimers Dement.

    (2011)
  • S.J. Vos et al.

    Preclinical Alzheimer's disease and its outcome: a longitudinal cohort study

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2013)
  • M. Wirth et al.

    The effect of amyloid β on cognitive decline is modulated by neural integrity in cognitively normal elderly

    Alzheimers Dement.

    (2013)
  • L. Yu et al.

    Disentangling the effects of age and APOE on neuropathology and late life cognitive decline

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2014)
  • N.D. Anderson et al.

    50 years of cognitive aging theory

    J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci.

    (2017)
  • J. Baker et al.

    Cognitive impairment and decline in cognitively normal older adults with high amyloid-β: a meta-analysis

    Alzheimers Dement.

    (2016)
  • D.A. Bennett et al.

    Relation of neuropathology to cognition in persons without cognitive impairment

    Ann. Neurol.

    (2012)
  • H. Braak et al.

    Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes

    Acta Neuropathol.

    (1991)
  • M.R. Brier et al.

    Tau and Aβ imaging, CSF measures, and cognition in Alzheimer's disease

    Sci. Transl. Med.

    (2016)
  • B.M. Brown et al.

    Influence of BDNF Val66Met on the relationship between physical activity and brain volume

    Neurology

    (2014)
  • C.L. Brown et al.

    Social activity and cognitive functioning over time: a coordinated analysis of four longitudinal studies

    J. Aging Res.

    (2012)
  • C. Bryant et al.

    The relationship between attitudes to aging and physical and mental health in older adults

    Int. Psychogeriatr.

    (2012)
  • L.R. Clark et al.

    Mild cognitive impairment in late middle age in the Wisconsin registry for Alzheimer's prevention study: prevalence and characteristics using robust and standard neuropsychological normative data

    Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.

    (2016)
  • I.J. Deary et al.

    Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging?

    Psychol. Aging

    (2010)
  • D. Delis et al.

    The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-kefs)

    (2001)
  • D. Delis et al.

    California Verbal Learning Test - Second Edition

    (2000)
  • D. Ebaid et al.

    Cognitive processing speed across the lifespan: beyond the influence of motor speed

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2017)
  • K.A. Ellis et al.

    The Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging: methodology and baseline characteristics of 1112 individuals recruited for a longitudinal study of Alzheimer's disease

    Int. Psychogeriatr.

    (2009)
  • B.E. Gavett et al.

    Practice effects on story memory and list learning tests in the neuropsychological assessment of older adults

    PLoS One

    (2016)
  • E. Grober et al.

    Conventional and robust norming in identifying preclinical dementia

    J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.

    (2015)
  • K.D. Harrington et al.

    Using robust normative data to investigate the neuropsychology of cognitive aging

    Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.

    (2017)
  • J. Hassenstab et al.

    Absence of practice effects in preclinical Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (2015)
  • D. Head et al.

    Neuroanatomical and cognitive mediators of age-related differences in episodic memory

    Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • P. Hedberg et al.

    Purpose in life among men and women aged 85 years and older

    Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev.

    (2010)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text