Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T10:17:29.960Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Emotional Contagion, Intimate Intercultural Relationships, and Intercultural Training

from Part IV - New Interdisciplinary Approaches to Intercultural Training

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Dan Landis
Affiliation:
University of Hawaii, Hilo
Dharm P. S. Bhawuk
Affiliation:
University of Hawaii, Manoa
Get access

Summary

In recent years, scholars from a wide variety of disciplines have begun to study the process of emotional contagion. These disciplines include cultural psychology, anthropology, primatology, the neurosciences, biology, social psychology, and history. Primitive emotional contagion appears to be a basic building block of human interaction. It assists in “mind-reading” (allowing people to understand others’ thinking), sharing others’ emotions, as well as coordinating and synchronizing their activities with others.Primitive emotional contagion is also an important component of empathy. In this chapter we will discuss the many ways people can “mind-read” and feel themselves into others’ emotional experiences – especially those from disparate cultures and ethnic groups. We will also discuss the ways in which an understanding of the contagion process may be integrated into intercultural training programs. We begin by reviewing the theory of Emotional Contagion.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelmann, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Facial efference and the experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 249280.Google Scholar
Ahmed, S. (2004). The cultural politics of emotion. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2008). Emotional convergence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 10541068.Google Scholar
Aylward, A. G. (2008). Effect of context on mimicry and emotional contagion: Does disliking inhibit mimicry and emotional contagion? Thesis submitted to the University of Hawaii in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the MA in psychology. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Bennett, J., Bennett, M., & Landis, D. (2004). Introduction and overview. In Landis, D., Bennett, J. M., & Bennett, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (3rd ed., pp. 110). New York, NY: Sage.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. J., & Castiglioni, I. (2004). Embodied ethnocentrism and the feeling of culture. In Landis, D., Bennett, J. M., & Bennett, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (3rd ed., pp. 249269). New York, NY: Sage.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher–student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 120138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Knee, C. R., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the social affordance of rapport: A validation study. Unpublished manuscript, Oregon State University, Corvallis.Google Scholar
Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2011). Spirituality and Indian psychology: Lessons from the Bhagavad-Gita. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Bhawuk, D. P. S., Carr, S. C., Gloss, A. E., Foster, L., & Thompson, L. (2014). Poverty reduction through positive work cycles: Exploring the role of information about work, culture, and diversity, and organizational justice. In Barriers to and opportunities for poverty reduction: Prospects for private sector-led interventions (pp. 6394). Istanbul: Istanbul International Center for Private Sector in Development.Google Scholar
Brennan, T. (2004). The transmission of affect. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cappella, J. N., & Planalp, S. (1981). Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations: III. Interspeaker influence. Human Communication Research, 7, 117132.Google Scholar
Carey, B. (February 12, 2008). You remind me of me: Decoding the subtle cues that lead to human rapport, scientists train their focus on mimicry. The New York Times: Science Times. pp. D1 and D6.Google Scholar
Carr, E. W., Winkielman, P., & Oveis, (2013). Transforming the mirror: Power fundamentally changes facial responding to emotional expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 17. DOI: 10.1037/a0034972Google Scholar
Chapple, E. D. (1982). Movement and sound: The musical language of body rhythms in interaction. In Davis, M. (Ed.), Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior (pp. 3152). New York, NY: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In. Hassan, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The new unconscious. Oxford series in social cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 334361). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1966). Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 338347.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of social reasoning. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 12591270). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872/2005). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Whitefish, MT: Kessinger.Google Scholar
Davis, M. R. (1985). Perceptual and affective reverberation components. In Goldstein, A. B. & Michaels, G. Y. (Eds.), Empathy: Development, training, and consequences (pp. 62108). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Doherty, R. W. (1997). The emotional contagion scale: A measure of individual differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 131154.Google Scholar
Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & Van Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100108.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 12081210.Google Scholar
Fowler, S. M., & Blohm, J. M. (2004). An analysis of methods for intercultural training. In Landis, D., Bennett, J. M., & Bennett, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (3rd ed., pp. 3784). New York, NY: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grusec, J. E., & Abramovitch, R. (1982). Imitation of peers and adults in a natural setting: A functional analysis. Child Development, 53, 636642.Google Scholar
Hall, J. A. (1979). Gender, gender roles, and nonverbal communication skills. In Rosenthal, R. (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences (pp. 32–67). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Bensman, L., Thornton, P. D., & Rapson, R. L. (in press). New perspectives on emotional contagion: A review of classic and recent research on facial mimicry and contagion. Interpersona.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Bensman, L., Thornton, P. D., & Rapson, R. L. (2014). Emotional mimicry and emotional contagion in social context. In Hess, U. & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Emotional mimicry in social context. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. (1992). Emotional contagion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Hsee, C. K., Costello, J., Weisman, M. S., & Denney, C. (1995). The impact of vocal feedback on emotional experience and expression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 293312.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Rapson, R. L., & Le, Y. L. (2009). Primitive emotional contagion: Recent research. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy. Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hawk, S. T. (2014). Cross-channel mimicry. In Hess, U. & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Emotional mimicry in social context. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hawk, S. T., Fischer, A. H., & Van Kleff, G. A. (2012). Face the noise: Embodied responses to nonverbal vocalizations of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 796814.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 129141.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2014) Emotional mimicry in social context. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hsee, C, K., Hatfield, E., Carlson, J. G., & Chemtob, C. (1990). The effect of power on susceptibility to emotional contagion. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 327340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., & Chemtob, C. (1992). Assessments of emotional states of others: Conscious judgments versus emotional contagion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 2, 119128.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding others: Imitation, language, and empathy. In Hurley, S. & Chater, N., Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science: Vol. 1. Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals (pp. 77101). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kimura, M., Daibo, I., & Yogo, M. (2008). The study of emotional contagion from the perspective of interpersonal relationships. Social Behavior and Personality, 36, 2742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaFrance, M. (1982). Posture mirroring and rapport. In Davis, M. (Ed.), Interaction Rhythms: Periodicity in communication behavior (pp. 279298). New York, NY: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Laird, J. D., & Bresler, C. (1992). The process of emotional experience: A self-perception theory. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 13. Emotion (pp. 213234). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334339.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145162.Google Scholar
Lundqvist, L. O. (1995). Facial EMG reactions to facial expressions: A case of facial emotional contagion? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 36, 130141.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R. (1988). The role of facial movement in emotion. In Wagner, H. L. (Ed.), Social psychophysiology and emotion: Theory and clinical applications (pp. 105–130). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253.Google Scholar
McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., Sullivan, D. G., Masters, R. D., & Englis, B. G. (1985). Emotional reactions to a political leader’s expressive displays. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 15131529.Google Scholar
Miller, J. B. (1976). Towards a new psychology of women. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Moore, S. ( 1984). The Stanislavski System. New York, NY: Viking Press.Google Scholar
Morris, D. (1966). Postural echo: Manwatching (pp. 83–85). New York, NY: Henry N. Abrahams.Google Scholar
Neal, D. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: Amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 673678. DOI: 10.1177/1948550611406138Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Poe, E. A. (1915). The purloined letter. The tales and poems of Edgar Allan Poe: Vol. III. Tales and poems (pp. 84113). New York, NY: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. (2005). The mirror neuron system and imitation. In Hurley, S. & Chater, N., Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science: Vol. 1. Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals (pp. 5576). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sato, W., Fujimura, T., & Suzuki, N. (2008). Enhanced facial EMG activity in response to dynamic facial expressions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70, 7074. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.%202008.%2006.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singelis, T. M. (1995). The effects of culture, gender, and self-construal on emotional contagion. A dissertation submitted to the University of Hawaii in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a PhD degree in Psychology. Honolulu: University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1759/1976). The theory of moral sentiments. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women’s intuition: The effect of subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 146155.Google Scholar
Stockert, N. (1994). Perceived similarity and emotional contagion. A PhD dissertation in partial fulfillment of the requirements of a PhD in Psychology. University of Hawaii, Honolulu, HI.Google Scholar
Thomas, D. L., Franks, D. D., & Calonico, J. M. (1972). Role-taking and power in social psychology. American Sociological Review, 37, 605614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, P. D. (2014). The effect of expectations on the susceptibility to emotional contagion. (Unpublished Master of Arts thesis). University of Hawaii, Honolulu, HI.Google Scholar
Travis, F. (2012). Your brain is a river, not a rock. New York, NY: CreateSpace.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1975). Culture training, cognitive complexity, and interpersonal altitudes. In Brislin, R., Bochner, S., & Lonner, (Eds.) Cross-cultural perspectives on learning (pp. 39–78). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage and Wiley Halsted.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (2008). Foreword. In Landis, D., Bennett, J. M., & Bennett, M. J. (Eds.). Handbook of intercultural training (3rd ed., pp. ixxii). New York, NY: Sage.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: The emotions as social information (EASI) model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 184188. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01633.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Pietroni, D., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2006). European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 557581. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitz, S. (1974). Nonverbal communication: Readings with commentary. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weyers, P., Hess, U., Hühnerl, I. (2014). Sociality of the interaction partner. In Hess, U. & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Emotional mimicry in social context. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wild, B., Erb, M., Eyb, M., Bartels, M., & Grodd, W. (2003). Why are smiles contagious? An fMRI study of the interaction between perception of facial affect and facial movements. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 123, 1736.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×